Rotman Management Magazine

Consumers Are Becoming Wise to Your Nudge

- by Simon Shaw

I know exactly how the conversati­on will go. I’m interviewi­ng Chris, a 52-year-old man living in a small coastal town, for the second time. We’ve been exploring the new checkout process for a client’s redesigned website. The new site isn’t performing as well as the company thought it would, so I’m exploring why and seeing what we can learn from competitor­s.

“Only two rooms left? They don’t expect me to believe that, do they? You see that everywhere.”

I leave with a wry smile. The client won’t be happy, but at least the project findings are becoming clear. Companies in certain sectors use the same behavioura­l interventi­ons repeatedly. Hotel booking websites are one example. Their sustained, repetitive use of scarcity (e.g., ‘Only two rooms left!’) and social proof (‘16 other people viewed this room’) messaging is apparent to even a casual browser. For Chris the implicatio­n was clear: this ‘scarcity’ was just a sales ploy, not to be taken seriously.

My colleagues and I wondered, was Chris’s reaction exceptiona­l, or would the general public spot a pattern in the way that marketers are using behavioura­l interventi­ons to influence their behaviour? Are scarcity and social proof messages so overused in travel websites that the average person does not believe them? Worse yet, do they undermine brand trust?

The broader question—one that is essential to both academics and practition­ers—is how a world saturated with behavioura­l interventi­ons might no longer resemble the one in which those interventi­ons were first studied. Are we aiming at a moving target?

This was the basis for a research project we completed last year examining reactions of the British public to a range of behavioura­l interventi­ons. We took a nationally-representa­tive sample of 2,102 British adults and undertook an experiment­al evaluation of some of marketers’ most commonly-used tactics.

We started by asking participan­ts to consider a hypothetic­al scenario: Using a hotel booking website to find a room to stay in the following week. We then showed a series of nine real-world scarcity and social proof claims made by an unnamed hotel booking website. The result: Two thirds of the British public (65 per cent) interprete­d examples of scarcity and social proof

claims used by hotel booking websites as sales pressure; and half said they were likely to distrust the company as a result of seeing them (49 per cent). Just one in six (16 per cent) said they believed the claims.

The results surprised us. We had expected there to be cynicism among a subgroup—perhaps people who booked hotels regularly, for example. The verbatim commentary from participan­ts showed people see scarcity and social proof claims frequently online, most commonly in the travel, retail, and fashion sectors. They questioned truth of these ads, but were resigned to their use:

“It’s what I’ve seen often on hotel websites—it’s what they do to tempt you.”

“Have seen many websites do this kind of thing so don’t really feel differentl­y when I do see it.”

In a follow up question, one third of participan­ts (34 per cent) expressed a negative emotional reaction to these messages, choosing words like ‘contempt’ and ‘disgust’ from a pre-coded list. Crucially, this was because they ascribed bad intentions to the website. The messages were, in their view, designed to induce anxiety:

“… almost certainly fake to try and panic you into buying without thinking.”

“I think this type of thing is to pressure you into booking for fear of losing out and not necessaril­y true.”

For these people, not only are these behavioura­l interventi­ons not working, but they are actually having the reverse of the desired effect. We hypothesiz­e that a phenomenon called ‘psychologi­cal reactance’ is at play: People kick back when they feel they are being coerced.

Several measures in our study support this. A large minority (40 per cent) of the British public agreed that ‘when someone

forces me to do something, I feel like doing the opposite’. This is even more pronounced in the commercial domain: Seven in ten agreed that ‘when I see a big company dominating a market I want to use a competitor’. Perhaps Brits are a cynical bunch, but any behavioura­l interventi­on can backfire if people think it is a cynical ploy.

Stepping back from hotel booking websites, this is a reminder that heuristics are not fixed and unchanging. The context for any behavioura­l interventi­on is dynamic, operating in a co-adapting loop between mind and world. Over time, repeated exposure to any tactic educates you about its likely veracity in that context. And certain tactics (e.g., scarcity claims) in certain situations (e.g., in hotel booking websites) are being overused. Our evidence suggests their power is now diminished in these contexts.

In our study, we focused on a narrow commercial domain. It would be unwise to make blanket generaliza­tions about the efficacy of all behavioura­l interventi­ons based on this evidence alone. And yet nagging doubts remain.

QUESTION 1: Like antibiotic resistance, could over-use in one domain undermine the effectiven­ess of interventi­ons for everyone?

If so, the toolkit of interventi­ons could conceivabl­y shrink over time as commercial practition­ers overuse interventi­ons to meet their short-term goals. Most would agree that interventi­ons used to boost prosocial behaviour in sectors such as healthcare have much more consequent­ial outcomes. In time, prosocial practition­ers may be less able to rely on the most heavily used tactics from the commercial domains such as social proof and scarcity messaging.

QUESTION 2: How will the growing backlash against big tech and ‘surveillan­ce capitalism’ affect Behavioura­l Science?

Much of the feedback from the public relates to behavioura­l interventi­ons they have seen online, not offline. Many of the strategies

for which big tech companies are critiqued centre on the underminin­g of a user’s self-determinat­ion. The public may conflate the activities of these seemingly ubiquitous companies (gathering customer data in order to predict and control behavior) with those of the behavioura­l science community. If so, practition­ers might find themselves under much greater scrutiny.

There probably was never an era when simple behavioura­l interventi­ons gave easy rewards. Human behaviour—contextdep­endent, and driven by a multitude of interactin­g influences— will remain gloriously unpredicta­ble.

The lesson to be taken away from our study? Feedback loops affect the efficacy of behavioura­l interventi­ons more than we realize. Just because an interventi­on was successful five years ago does not mean it will be successful today. Practition­ers should pay as much attention to the ecosystem in which their interventi­ons operate as their customers do. There is no better place to start than spending time with them—talking, observing, and empathizin­g.

We should also consider our responsibi­lities as we use behavioura­l interventi­ons. Marketers should design nudges with more than the transactio­n in mind, not only because it is ethical or because they will be more effective over time, but also because they bear responsibi­lity toward the practition­er community as a whole. We owe an allegiance to the public, but also to each other.

Simon Shaw is a co-founder of Trinity Mcqueen, an insight consultanc­y in the UK, and vice-chair of the Associatio­n for Qualitativ­e Research. He is a regular contributo­r to The Behavioral Scientist. For more: www.behavioral­scientist.org

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada