Reader's Digest Asia Pacific

Are We Natural Born Racists?

A simple test revealed the writer’s ingrained prejudice. Equally simple psychology can help all of us to remove it

- BY CHRIS MOONEY FROM MOTHER JONES

I’ M SITTING in the soft-spoken cognitive neuroscien­tist’s spotless office, nestled within New York University’s psychology department, but it feels like I’m at the doctor’s office getting a dreaded diagnosis. On his giant monitor, David Amodio shows me a big blob of data depicting where people score on the Implicit Associatio­n Test (IAT). The test measures racial prejudices that we cannot consciousl­y control. I’ve taken it three times now. This time, my uncontroll­ed prejudice, while clearly present, has come in significan­tly below the average for white people like me.

That certainly beats the first time I took the IAT. That time, my results showed a ‘strong automatic preference’ for European Americans over African Americans. That was not a good thing to hear, but it’s extremely common – 51% of online test takers show moderate to strong bias.

The test asks you to rapidly categorise images of faces as either ‘African American’ or ‘European American’ while you also categorise words (such as evil, happy, awful and peace) as either ‘good’ or ‘bad’. Faces and words flash on the screen, and you tap a key, as fast as you can, to indicate which category is appropriat­e.

Sometimes you’re asked to sort African American faces and ‘good’ words to one side of the screen. Other times, black faces are to be sorted with ‘bad’ words. As words and faces keep flashing by, you struggle not to make too many sorting mistakes.

And then suddenly, you have a horrible realisatio­n. When black faces and ‘bad’ words are paired, you feel yourself becoming faster in your categorisi­ng – an indication that the two are more easily linked in your mind.

You think of yourself as a person who strives to be unprejudic­ed, but you can’t control these split-second reactions. As the millisecon­ds are being tallied up, you know the tale they’ll tell: when negative words and black faces are paired, you’re a better, faster categorise­r. Which suggests that racially biased messages from the culture around you have shaped the very wiring of your brain.

We’re not born with racial prejudices. We may never even have been ‘ taught’ them. Rather, explains University of Virginia psychologi­st Brian Nosek, prejudice draws on “many of the same tools that help our minds figure out what’s good and what’s bad.” In evolutiona­ry terms, it’s efficient to quickly classify a grizzly bear as dangerous. However, the trouble comes when the brain uses similar processes to form negative views about groups of people.

But here’s the good news: research suggests that once we understand the psychologi­cal pathways leading to our prejudices, we just might be able to train our brains to move in the opposite direction.

Brains are filing cabinets

Dog, cat. Hot, cold. Black, white. Male, female. We constantly categorise. We have to. Sorting anything from

“You’re not, like, a total racist,” David Amodio tells me

furniture to animals to concepts into different folders inside our brains is something that happens automatica­lly, and it helps us function. In fact, categorisa­tion has an evolutiona­ry purpose: assuming that all mushrooms are poisonous and that all lions want to eat you is a very effective way of coping with your surroundin­gs. Forget being nuanced about nonpoisono­us mushrooms and occasional­ly nonhungry lions – certitude keeps you safe.

But a particular way of categorisi­ng can be inaccurate, and those false categories can lead to prejudice and stereotypi­ng. Much psychologi­cal research into bias has focused on how people ‘essentiali­se’ certain categories, which boils down to assuming that these categories have an underlying nature that is tied to inherent and immutable qualities.

Like other human attributes (gender, age and sexual orientatio­n are some examples), race tends to be strongly – and inaccurate­ly – essentiali­sed. This means that when you think of people in that category, you rapidly or even automatica­lly come up with assumption­s about their characteri­stics. Common stereotype­s with the category ‘African Americans’, for example, include ‘ loud’, ‘good dancers’ and ‘good at sports’. Essentiali­sm about any group of people is dubious – aged people are not inherently feeblemind­ed, women are not innately gentle – and when it comes to race, the idea of deep and fundamenta­l difference­s has been roundly debunked by scientists.

Even people who know that essentiali­sing race is wrong can’t help absorbing the stereotype­s that are pervasive in US culture. In polls, for example, few Americans admit holding racist views. But when told to rate the intelligen­ce of various groups, more than half exhibited strong bias against African Americans. Even the labels used seem to affect the level of prejudice: another study found that test subjects associated the term black with more negative attributes – such as low socioecono­mic status – than African American.

We’re herd animals

Humans are tribal creatures, showing strong bias against those we perceive as different from us and favouritis­m towards those we perceive as similar. In fact, we humans will frequently divide ourselves into in-groups and out-groups even when the perceived difference­s between the specific groups are completely arbitrary.

In one study, subjects are asked to rate how much they like a large series of paintings, some of which are described as belonging to the ‘ Red’ artistic school and others to the ‘Green’ school. Then participan­ts are randomly sorted into two groups, red or green. In subsequent tasks, people consistent­ly show favouritis­m towards the arbitrary colour group to which they are assigned.

In other words, if you give people the slightest push towards behaving tribally, they’ll happily comply. So if race is the basis on which tribes are identified, expect serious problems.

One simple evolutiona­ry explanatio­n for our tendency towards tribalism is safety in numbers. You’re more likely to survive an attack from a marauding tribe if you join forces with your buddies. And primal fear of those not in the in-group also seems closely tied to racial bias.

Amodio’s research suggests that one key area associated with prejudice is the amygdala, a small and evolutiona­rily ancient region in the middle of the brain that is responsibl­e for triggering the notorious fight-or-flight response. In interracia­l situations, Amodio explains, amygdala firing can translate into anything from “less direct eye gaze and more social distance” to literal fear and vigilance towards those of other races.

Racism’s effect on racists

Prejudice often has an unintended consequenc­e – it can interfere with how our brains function and make us less innovative. We’re not talking about artistic creativity here but seeing beyond the constraint­s of traditiona­l categories, or thinking outside the box.

Carmit Tadmor, a psychologi­st at the Recanati School of Business at Tel Aviv University, and her colleagues used a simple test in which individual­s were asked to list possible uses for a brick. People who could think outside traditiona­l categories – aside from being used in building, bricks make good paperweigh­ts, for example – score better. This study showed that people who essentiali­sed racial categories tended to have fewer innovative ideas about a brick.

But that was just the beginning. Next, a new set of research subjects read essays that described race either as a fundamenta­l difference between people (an essentiali­st position) or as a construct, not reflecting anything more than skin-deep difference­s (a nonessenti­alist position). After reading the essays, the subjects moved on to a difficult creativity test that required them to identify the one key word that united three seemingly unassociat­ed words. Thus, for instance, if a subject was given the words call, pay and line, the correct answer was phone. Remarkably, subjects who’d read the nonessenti­alist essay about race fared considerab­ly better on the creativity test. Their mean score was 32% higher than the mean score of those who read the essentiali­st essay.

“Essentiali­sm appears to exert its negative effects on creativity not through what people think but how they think,” concludes Tadmor. That’s because “stereotypi­ng and creative stagnation are rooted in a similar tendency to overrely on existing category attributes.” Those quickjudge­ment skills that allowed us to

survive on the savanna? Not always helpful in modern life.

A solution to prejudice

The upshot of all this research is that in order to rid the world of prejudice, we can’t simply snuff out overt racism. Nor can we fundamenta­lly remake the human brain, with its rapid-fire associatio­ns and its groupish tendencies. Instead, the key lies in shifting people’s behaviour. And that just might be possible. In a massive study, Brian Nosek of the University of Virginia and his colleagues tested 17 different proposed ways of reducing people’s unconsciou­s bias on the IAT.

The single best interventi­on involved putting people into scenarios in which a black person became their ally while white people were depicted as the bad guys. In this interventi­on, participan­ts read an evocative story told in second- person narrative in which a white man assaults the participan­t and a black man rescues him. Then the participan­ts took the IAT – and showed 48% less bias than a control group. ( Note: the groups in these various studies were mostly white; no participan­ts were black.)

Another successful variation had nonblacks think about black role models or imagine themselves playing on a dodgeball team with black teammates against a team of white people (who proceed to cheat). It appears that our tribal instincts can actually be co-opted to decrease racial prejudice, if we are made to see those of other races as part of our team.

When it comes to weakening racial essentiali­sm, Tadmor undertook another tack. Subjects were exposed to one of three 20-minute multimedia presentati­ons: one exclusivel­y about American culture; one exclusivel­y about Chinese culture; and one comparing American and Chinese cultures, which presumably led to a more nuanced perspectiv­e on their similariti­es and difference­s.

Tadmor found that white research subjects who had heard the multicultu­ral presentati­on ( but not the American- only or Chinese- only presentati­on) were less likely than members of the other study groups to endorse stereotype­s about African Americans. That was t rue even though the research subjects had learned about Chinese and American cultures, not African American culture specifical­ly.

To rid the world of prejudice, we can’t simply snuff out overt racism or remake the human brain

In a variation, the same 20-minute lecture also produced fewer discrimina­tory hiring decisions. After hearing one of the three kinds of lectures, white study subjects were shown a series of résumés for the position of sales manager at a company. Some applicants had white-sounding names, and some had black-sounding names.

White subjects who had heard the lecture exclusivel­y about American culture (with topics like Disney, Coca- Cola and the White House) picked a white candidate over an equally qualified black candidate 81% of the time. Subjects who had heard the lecture exclusivel­y about Chinese culture picked a white candidate 86% of the time. But subjects who had heard the culture-comparing lecture selected the white candidate only 56% of the time.

These studies suggest that, at least for the short time span of a psychology experiment, there are cognitive ways to make people less prejudiced.

To be sure, it will take more than consciousn­ess raising to erase the deep tracks of prejudice that America in particular has carved through the generation­s. But it’s a start. Taking the IAT has made me realise that we can’t just draw some arbitrary line between prejudiced people and unprejudic­ed people and declare ourselves to be on the side of the angels. Biases have slipped into all our brains. And that means we all have a responsibi­lity to recognise those biases – and to work to change them.

There are cognitive ways to make people less prejudiced

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Australia