Gulf Today

Beware of former Rep. Beto O’rourke’s charisma

- Stephen L. Carter

Many Democrats are thrilled that former Rep. Beto O’rourke of Texas has tossed his hat into the presidenti­al ring. Ever since his near-upset of Sen. Ted Cruz last November, Beto (with whom we all seem to be on a first-name basis) has been a media darling — and scarcely a profile can be writen about him without mentioning his charisma.

“Charisma has propelled him far,” says Vox. “Beto’s been compared to Obama and been confused for a Kennedy.” A Forbes story details his “seven steps to charisma.” He’s “young and charismati­c,” says Politico.

You get the idea.

It’s easy to understand the longing for a charismati­c candidate. Crowds turn out, voters get excited _ young voters especially — and the next thing anyone knows, the election’s a done deal, with turnout effects all the way down the ticket.

But is charisma a good thing? Not according to Jessica Flanigan, a philosophe­r at the Jepson School at the University of Richmond. In a thoughtful 2013 paper titled “Charisma and Moral Reasoning,” she argues that the quality that is so regularly praised “is intrinsica­lly problemati­c from a moral perspectiv­e.”

How can this be, when people get so excited about charismati­c leadership? For Flanigan, the excitement itself turns out to be a problem. When people get excited about a leader, they tend to defer to that leader’s decisions — in effect, to indulge a presumptio­n that the leader will find the right answer. They do less reasoning on their own. “The problem with charisma,” she writes, “is either that it inspires followers to act rightly but for the wrong reasons, or worse, that it inspires people to act wrongly for the wrong reasons.”

Flanigan supports her arguments with extensive atention to social science. To take just one example, she cites a troubling 2009 study in which subjects were randomly assigned to different groups that were then asked to work cooperativ­ely to solve mathematic­s problems. The researcher­s found that the groups did not select as their leaders the members with the greatest mathematic­al ability; instead they selected for confidence and dominance of personalit­y.

In Flanigan’s view, it’s beter to follow, say, Martin Luther King Jr., because his argument is persuasive than because he’s a “commanding and impressive” speaker. Ater all, even a commanding and impressive speaker can be wrong. Thus, she concludes, we should resist the appeal of charismati­c leadership and search for other qualities instead.

Maybe you’re less troubled than Flanigan about the prospect that people might do the right thing because the charismati­c leader says so rather than because they’ve figured it out for themselves. But if you believe that a selfgovern­ing polity ought to reason and deliberate, then she has a point.

In any case, we should all be worried about the other possibilit­y: that because the charismati­c leader says so, supporters will think the wrong thing is right. If you doubt that people behave this way, ask yourself why product marketers and political candidates alike so assiduousl­y seek out celebrity endorsers.

Another charisma skeptic is the psychologi­st Tomas Chamorro-premuzic. Writing in 2012 in the Harvard Business Review, he makes a case similar to Flanigan’s, but with a particular point in mind: “There are only three ways to influence others: force, reason, or charm. Whereas force and reason are rational (even when we are ‹forced’ to do something, we obey for a good reason) charm is not.” When we are charmed by a charismati­c leader, we may follow without reasons. The charm, rather than the mind or the morals, becomes the reason for the leader’s success.

 ??  ?? Beto O›rourke
Beto O›rourke
 ??  ?? Barack Obama
Barack Obama

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Bahrain