Ottawa Citizen

AT YOUR SERVICE

Study aims to get more pups trained to help humans

- KARIN BRULLIARD

DURHAM, N.C. Yonder, 11 weeks old and 15 pounds, had two choices. In a white-walled room at Duke University, the wiggly Labrador mix faced a neon green squeaky squid toy and an upturned bowl topped with a piece of kibble. “OK!” a researcher says, and the puppy didn’t hesitate — she scurried straight toward the treat.

Yonder was bred for an exceptiona­lly difficult job: to become a service dog for a human who needs her — by alerting to a doorbell or pulling a wheelchair while remaining composed and quiet, in crowds or on trails, and never chasing squirrels. Whether she’s capable was being gleaned in this room, with tests aimed at measuring her problem-solving, self-control and communicat­ions with people.

That was the hope, at least, for Yonder and her six furry cohorts. Early this year, they were the newest subjects of a US$1.6 million study, funded by the National Institutes of Health, to help untangle a question long asked by breeders and trainers and now increasing­ly scrutinize­d by scientists: What makes a successful service dog — and can it be predicted in a puppy as young as Yonder?

At stake is a lot of money and a lot of dogs. Our expanding understand­ing of canines’ unique skills has fuelled interest in service dogs among people with disabiliti­es and the military, but it has also spawned scammers and yearslong wait-lists. Although large organizati­ons have honed the use of breeding and training to produce calm and obedient dogs, only about 50 per cent make the cut. By that time, nearly two years and as much as $50,000 have been spent on one dog.

That is where another booming field — canine science — is coming in. Over two decades, the study of dog minds, genetics and behaviour has given rise to laboratori­es at universiti­es around the world. And in service dog organizati­ons, with their controlled breeding and noble missions, canine researcher­s see ideal study population­s.

“We’re trying to understand the dog side of the leash and how we get more dogs helping more people,” said Brian Hare, an evolutiona­ry anthropolo­gist who is co-director of the Duke Canine Cognition Center, which is studying puppies bred by the California-based Canine Companions for Independen­ce (CCI).

Some discoverie­s have been made already. Hare and a colleague found that successful service dogs more often make eye contact with a person when facing an unsolvable task and use inferentia­l reasoning to find a hidden reward. Another study concluded that puppies with “helicopter moms” are more likely to fail as guide dogs, while young dogs that quickly solve a multistep problem are more likely to succeed. A neuroscien­tist who scanned the brains of service dog candidates found that washouts had higher activity in the area associated with excitabili­ty.

Other revelation­s are further off, but potentiall­y revolution­ary. One canine geneticist is collecting thousands of DNA samples in a bid to pinpoint the genetic markers of star service and working dogs.

“It’s huge, huge,” Brenda Kennedy, CCI’s director of canine health and research, said of the impact the research could have on a donor-funded group like hers, which provides dogs at no cost. “It really comes down to numbers. Every time we increase the percentage of dogs that succeed in our program, the more we’re going to be able to have an impact.”

That is why Yonder and her peers — Arthur, Aurora, Westley, Wisdom, Zindel and Zola — were enrolled in a sort of boarding school for future service dogs on the campus of one of the nation’s elite universiti­es. Duke calls it “puppy kindergart­en.”

Pups that make it will become one of the five kinds of service dogs CCI provides, which include hearing dogs and assistance dogs for veterans with PTSD. Washouts might be “released” to another organizati­on for a different job, but often they become pets.

In March, the spread of the coronaviru­s forced Duke to close, and Yonder’s cohort was sent to live in private homes. But the research has continued with puppies being raised off-campus, and Hare said he expects the kindergart­en to resume at the university in 2021.

In normal times, this is how it works: A new group of puppies arrives each semester and bunks, for 12 weeks, in the brick biological sciences building home to the Canine Cognition Center or in dorms with students. During the day, all romp together around a linoleum-floored room that amounts to a puppy daycare, with plush dog beds, soft lighting, birch tree decals on a wall and a white noise machine playing forest sounds. Outdoors is an artificial turf play area, where the sight of tussling puppies regularly stops passersby in their tracks.

The puppies are cared for and cuddled by student volunteers, who were, unsurprisi­ngly, eager to help. (Six hundred — one-tenth of the undergradu­ate student body — responded to a call but were winnowed down to 150 after being required to take a five-hour online course about dog cognition and attending a meeting, Hare said.)

The pups face 14 cognitive tests every two weeks from the time they are eight weeks to 20 weeks old, the most rapid period of brain developmen­t. At 16 weeks, Hare said, their brains are the equivalent of a six-year-old child’s.

Hare has been working with CCI for about a decade, since, he said, he was shocked to learn at a conference that behaviouri­sm — the idea that a person or animal’s behaviour can be explained or altered by conditioni­ng — was still canon among dog trainers.

Hare said he and other scientists have already found, for a study not yet published, that puppies’ performanc­e on some tests at 10 weeks mirrors how they do at 18 weeks. The Duke project aims to get even greater “resolution” on when these skills develop, Hare said, and how early they predict later success.

Innate skills are not everything. To an unknown degree, environmen­t matters, too, and another side of puppy kindergart­en is a socializat­ion experiment. Might an amped-up social environmen­t in these formative weeks provide a sort of “head start,” as Hare puts it?

Before starting training at 18 months, most service dogs are raised in homes by individual or family “puppy raisers.”

The Duke puppies are being raised around one another and a stream of humans. Any student can visit the nursery and cavort with puppies, and 4,000 did so last fall. The puppies visit pediatric patients at Duke University Hospital. Medical students perform exams on the pups, as practice for interactin­g with non-verbal young children.

“When we see him starting to get frustrated, we can just re-engage him,” Margaret Gruen, an assistant professor at North Carolina State University’s College of Veterinary Medicine, said as she held Zindel, a yellow Lab, on an exam table in the Duke cognition lab. Next to Gruen, who is co-directing the research

I’m not out to breed couch potatoes, even though they’re lovely pets. It’s really that we want as many guide dogs as possible.

at Duke, medical student Laura Noteware gently felt for Zindel’s lymph nodes.

“Good boy! Good job! Lovely lymph nodes,” Noteware said. Zindel, sitting calmly for the prodding, eagerly gobbled a treat.

Service dog providers have long used behaviour and temperamen­t tests — to measure fearfulnes­s, say, or aggression — during puppyhood and training. But there’s been little large-scale data collection or consistenc­y, researcher­s say.

“There are programs that change their policies with the wind — this year it’s Test A, and this year it’s Test B,” said Evan MacLean, who directs the canine cognition centre at the University of Arizona and frequently collaborat­es with Hare. He said he’s optimistic the wave of research will boost providers’ success rates, but he warned that it will take time.

Some providers that breed puppies also use genetics, analyzing pedigrees to estimate the likelihood that a breeding pair will pass along certain traits, such as hip dysplasia or fear of thunder. By collecting this informatio­n over three decades, the New Yorkbased Guiding Eyes for the Blind, which provides seeing-eye dogs, has raised its success rate from about 20 per cent of puppies born to nearly 40 per cent, said Jane Russenberg­er, its senior director for breeding and genetics. (Dogs’ most common reason for failure, she said, is not being able to bounce back into work mode after something alarming occurs.)

But each year, about 170 puppies graduate from the program, and about 400 applicatio­ns for dogs come in, she said. Its wait list is about 150 people long. That is why Guiding Eyes is now working with Elinor Karlsson, a geneticist at the Broad Institute of the Massachuse­tts Institute of Technology and Harvard.

Karlsson studies the genetics of dog behaviour, and she sees working dogs — a group that includes service dogs and those that do jobs like drug detection — as key subjects, because they are mostly selected for behaviour, not looks. By sequencing working dogs’ DNA, she hopes to discover patterns that correlate with success. She figures she needs samples from 10,000 dogs to make this work; she has about 1,600.

“If you had a predictive test that would be able to tell you whether a dog is likely to be successful, you could do that as a puppy and decide whether you want to invest the resources,” or even test shelter dogs, Karlsson said.

Russenberg­er’s bar is lower. Her hope is that Karlsson’s work will help Guiding Eyes hone its selection of breeding dogs, leading to higher graduation rates.

“I’m not out to breed couch potatoes, even though they’re lovely pets. It’s really that we want as many guide dogs as possible,” she said. “Just think of the years of savings by being able to serve more people who are blind and visually impaired.”

The Washington Post

 ?? PHOTOS: JEREMY M. LANGE/FOR THE WASHINGTON POST ?? Medical student Laura Noteware examines service-dog candidate Zindel at the Duke University Canine Cognition Center.
PHOTOS: JEREMY M. LANGE/FOR THE WASHINGTON POST Medical student Laura Noteware examines service-dog candidate Zindel at the Duke University Canine Cognition Center.
 ??  ?? Puppies engage in a friendly tug-of-war competitio­n in the play area.
Puppies engage in a friendly tug-of-war competitio­n in the play area.
 ??  ?? “We’re trying to understand the dog side of the leash,” says co-director Brian Hare, shown with research scientist Vanessa Woods.
“We’re trying to understand the dog side of the leash,” says co-director Brian Hare, shown with research scientist Vanessa Woods.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada