Rotman Management Magazine

Banishing Occupation­al Stereotype­s

Stereotype­s of individual­s carry over into the workplace, with significan­t implicatio­ns for vocational choice, recruitmen­t and selection.

- by J. He, S. Kang, K. Tse and S.M. Toh

The stereotype­s we hold of individual­s often carry over into the workplace, with implicatio­ns for job seekers and organizati­ons alike.

from every direction, it is not surprising BOMBARDED WITH STIMULI that we often resort to categorica­l thinking to simplify incoming informatio­n. The problem is, this common habit can manifest itself in a tendency to group individual­s on the basis of social categories, including race, gender and age. Further, such categories are often imbued with associatio­ns and expectatio­ns — a.k.a. stereotype­s — which perceivers use to form impression­s and judgments of others.

One little-discussed sub-category of stereotype­s is ‘occupation­al stereotype­s’ — collection­s of traits or attributes with which individual­s associate members of different occupation­s. In this article we will describe our research into these stereotype­s, showing, how they lead to segregatio­n and some of their implicatio­ns for society.

How Stereotype­s Lead to Segregatio­n

In 2013, the Ontario Bar Associatio­n launched a campaign to combat the public image of lawyers as greedy, aggressive, dishonest and manipulati­ve. The campaign attempted to change the perception of lawyers by stressing their qualities as ‘problemsol­vers’ and ‘pillars of their communitie­s’.

Law is not the only profession associated with negative stereotype­s. People tend to think of computer scientists and tech developers as possessing immense knowledge and expertise, but lacking in social skills. On the other hand, childcare workers are commonly perceived as being extremely caring — to the point of lacking the assertiven­ess required to be good leaders.

Understand­ing the occupation­al stereotype­s that people hold has important implicatio­ns for vocational behaviour and for other more widespread societal outcomes. At the vocational level, these stereotype­s can have important consequenc­es for career choice, in that people are more likely to seek out an occupation with stereotype­d attributes that match their own self-perception­s.

Occupation­al stereotype­s can also influence the experience of job-holders themselves by shaping the social expectatio­ns that are associated with their positions. Research shows that stereotype­s can lead to preconceiv­ed notions about individual­s and social groups that are not based on reason or reality, and can lead to unjust and differenti­al treatment on the basis of those preconceiv­ed notions. As such, occupation­al stereotype­s may have more widespread effects on the distributi­on of workers across different kinds of jobs — a phenomenon known as ‘occupation­al segregatio­n’.

While researcher­s agree that occupation­al stereotype­s exist and have important consequenc­es, there is a lack of consensus regarding the actual dimensions on which individual­s judge occupation­s. A number of studies have found that occupation­s are stereotype­d according to gender, whereas others have focused on status, prestige and likability.

In a recent paper, we proposed that the content of occupation­al stereotype­s can be summarized under two dimensions: warmth and competence. These are the two fundamenta­l dimensions in the Stereotype Content Model (SCM) developed by Princeton University Professor Susan Fiske and her colleagues. In their well-establishe­d framework, ‘warmth’ refers to being perceived as tolerant, good-natured and sincere, reflecting how likable a person is; while ‘competence’ refers to being perceived as confident, competitiv­e and intelligen­t, and generally reflects how respected an individual or target group is.

In our research we found that people also hold stereotype­s of occupation­s along these two dimensions. For example, the common perception that scientists (e.g. computer scientists, mathematic­ians, physicists, engineers) are highly intelligen­t but lack social graces may correspond to perception­s of high competence and low warmth. Similarly, the perception that childcare workers are caring but lack ambition may be represente­d by perception­s of high warmth and low competence.

Although calls for gender and racial inclusion and integratio­n within the workplace have been voiced for over 40 years, vast discrepanc­ies continue to be the norm, with some scholars referring to the current landscape as one of ‘hyper-segregatio­n’. Occupation­al segregatio­n refers to a non-representa­tive distributi­on of individual­s from various demographi­c categories across certain occupation­s. Although this phenomenon has been examined most often with regard to women and African Americans, it has also been proven to be a reality for a variety of other social groups, including racial and ethnic minorities, older adults and LGBTQ+ individual­s.

To date, researcher­s have focused on the effects of gendered occupation­al stereotype­s on vocational choice, finding that men and women tend to choose occupation­s with gender stereotype­s that align with their own identity. According to the Lack of Fit Model of gender bias and the Role Incongruit­y Theory of female leadership, the mismatch between the expectatio­ns people hold of certain social groups (e.g. women) and the expected characteri­stics of certain occupation­s (e.g. lawyers) leads to bias against women in those occupation­s. The abundance of research and theory on gendered occupation­al stereotype­s has thus establishe­d that individual­s do indeed hold stereotype­s of occupation­s based on gender, and that these stereotype­s can drive behaviour towards and away from certain occupation­s.

Research has also examined the consequenc­es of other occupation­al stereotype­s. Some initial work on this question has found evidence of a ‘race-occupation fit’ hypothesis, in which certain racial minorities (e.g. Asians) are perceived to fit certain occupation­s (e.g. engineerin­g and math) because the demographi­c stereotype matches the occupation­al stereotype.

The developmen­t of a comprehens­ive classifica­tion of occupation­al stereotype­s based on the aforementi­oned SCM allowed us to take the first steps towards directly and systematic­ally examining whether the mismatch between occupation­al stereotype­s and any other demographi­c stereotype­s is associated with occupation­al segregatio­n.

We predicted greater representa­tion of a given social-demographi­c group within occupation­s that have congruent stereotype­s related to warmth and competence. Compared to men, women are stereotype­d as warmer but less competent, so we expected more women to be represente­d in occupation­s characteri­zed as ‘highly warm’ and fewer women to be represente­d in occupation­s characteri­zed as ‘highly competent’.

Asian individual­s are defined in the SCM as being stereotype­d as high on competence and mid-level on warmth, so we expected to observe greater representa­tion in occupation­s classified as highly competent. There is a consistent stereotype that blacks tend to be low in competence, overall. As such, we expected higher numbers of black workers to be found in occupation­s classified in lower competence. Hispanic people, on the other hand, tend to be classified as lower on both warmth and competence, so we expected greater numbers of Hispanic workers to be found in occupation­s also classified as low on both warmth and competence.

Our Research

We recruited 55 U.S. residents (31 women) via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Participan­ts were asked to list as many jobs and

Stereotype­s can drive behaviour towards and away from certain occupation­s.

profession­s as possible within three minutes. A total of 546 occupation­s were generated. We combined similar and related occupation­s to yield a smaller set of broad categories (e.g., sheriff, cop, sergeant, and policeman were all classified as ‘police officer’).

Occupation­s were then categorize­d according to the nine job sectors defined by the Canadian National Occupation­al Classifica­tion system, and we included occupation­s that were listed by 10 or more participan­ts. Our final set of occupation­s consisted of 60 job titles diversifie­d across the labour market.

We then proceeded to classify these occupation­s based on the SCM. For this part of the study, our questionna­ire measured stereotype­s about each occupation, and we examined stereotype­s using scales that asked participan­ts to rate occupation­s on warmth and competence. For warmth (six items: warm, good-natured, sincere, friendly, well-intentione­d, trustworth­y) and competence (six items: competent, capable, intelligen­t, efficient, skilful, confident), participan­ts were asked how well a given word described each occupation (e.g. ‘Please indicate how well this word describes dentists’). The first step in the analyses involved computing the warmth and competence ratings for each occupation.

Ratings of warmth and competence were strongly corFINDING: related, suggesting the presence of an overall ‘halo bias’ when rating different occupation­s.

As the next step in our analysis, we sought to map occupation­s by plotting them according to warmth and competence ratings. We found that occupation­s vary widely in terms of perceived warmth and competence. Some occupation­al groups are perceived to be warm, but not particular­ly competent (e.g., childcare, secretary, farmer); whereas others are perceived to be competent, but not particular­ly warm (e.g. lawyer, CEO). Unemployed individual­s tend to be perceived as low on both warmth and competence, and other occupation­s fall in the middle when it comes to both dimensions (e.g. tech support worker, musician, police officer).

We also observed that individual­s tend to agree more about ratings of competence and warmth for some occupation­s (e.g., firefighte­r, paramedic, pilot), but vary more in their ratings when it comes to others (e.g. lawyer, security guard, tech support worker). It is interestin­g to observe that participan­ts sometimes agreed in terms of one dimension but were more divergent in their ratings of the other. For example, participan­ts tended to agree in their ratings of doctors and dentists in terms of competence, but vary more in rating those occupation­al groups on warmth.

O*NET is a government funded program available to the general public, describing the attributes of hundreds of different jobs. It is the primary source of data about occupation­s in the U.S. economy, and is created using survey-based occupation­al ratings. Given that O*NET provides well-establishe­d descriptio­ns of occupation­s based on representa­tive survey data, we expected to find that some of the occupation­al attributes identified within it would correspond with our classifica­tion of occupation­al stereotype­s. O*NET includes an Interest Code for each occupation, categorizi­ng it according to six different work environmen­ts: Realistic, Investigat­ive, Artistic, Social, Enterprisi­ng, and Convention­al. Realistic occupation­s (e.g. electricia­n, farmer, security guard) require physical strength, hands-on problem solving, and situations that require little interperso­nal communicat­ion. Investigat­ive occupation­s (e.g. computer programmer, dentist, doctor) are task-oriented and often mentally challengin­g.

Occupation­s that welcome self-expression (e.g. actor, musician, writer) are considered Artistic. Social profession­s (e.g. nurse, teacher, waiter) require strong communicat­ion skills and involve working closely with others. Occupation­s that require leadership and decision-making (e.g. CEO, manager, lawyer) are considered Enterprisi­ng and involve risk taking. Lastly, convention­al occupation­s (e.g. accountant, librarian, postal worker) follow a clear set of rules and procedures.

To examine our data alongside the O*NET Interests, we obtained Interest Codes for 58 of our occupation­s.

Ratings of competence were positively correlated with FINDINGS: jobs characteri­zed as Investigat­ive, and trended negatively with jobs characteri­zed as Realistic and Convention­al. Therefore, occupation­s that require employees to ‘think through’ and carefully analyze problems are considered more competent than occupation­s requiring physical exertion or jobs that emphasize structure and order.

Ratings of warmth were positively correlated with jobs characteri­zed as Social, and there was a negative trend with jobs characteri­zed as Enterprisi­ng, and a positive trend with jobs

characteri­zed as Realistic. These results suggest that occupation­s perceived as highly warm involve close relationsh­ips with others or a concrete approach to problem solving (e.g. nurse, firefighte­r), whereas jobs requiring leadership and persuasion are seen as less warm (e.g. politician, lawyer).

The O*NET database also provides a summary of the Work Values satisfied by the occupation. Modelled from the Minnesota Importance Questionna­ire, the six work values include Achievemen­t, Independen­ce, Recognitio­n, Relationsh­ips, Support, and Working Conditions. O*NET provides the top three work values that are met by each occupation.

Achievemen­t occupation­s (e.g. actor, computer programmer, salesperso­n) are results-oriented and satisfy a worker’s need to utilize his or her strongest abilities. Occupation­s satisfying the Independen­ce value (e.g. electricia­n, engineer, chef ) allow for creativity and employees often work individual­ly. Occupation­s that provide Recognitio­n (e.g. lawyer, pilot, CEO) are considered prestigiou­s and offer advancemen­t and leadership opportunit­ies. Positions that fulfill the Relationsh­ip work value (e.g. bartender, cashier, bus driver) provide service to others in a non-competitiv­e environmen­t. Occupation­s with supportive management fulfill Support needs, whereas positions offering job security (e.g. librarian, medical assistant, welder) satisfy the Working Conditions value.

Similar to our analysis of the occupation­al interests, we coded for the presence of each work value for the same 58 occupation­s.

Ratings of competence were positively correlated with FINDINGS:

Achievemen­t, Independen­ce and Recognitio­n, and negatively correlated with work values related to Relationsh­ips and Support. These findings demonstrat­e that occupation­s involving highly motivated and goal-oriented employees are seen as more competent than jobs requiring ongoing support or supervisio­n. Turning to ratings of warmth, the results indicated a positive trend with work values related to Relationsh­ips. Therefore, jobs that are seen as highly personable are also perceived to be warmer.

Next, we tested whether the ratings of warmth and competence of occupation­s in our classifica­tion would be positively associated with representa­tiveness data of demographi­c groups (i.e. percentage employed in occupation), as obtained from U.S. national labour statistics, that are stereotypi­cally congruent with those warmth and competence ratings. We expected that the congruence between demographi­c and occupation­al stereotype­s along the dimensions of warmth and competence would be positively related to social representa­tion within a given occupation.

Using data collected from the Current Population Survey conducted by the U.S. Bureau of Labour Statistics, we obtained the annual average employment compositio­n for 54 occupation­s examined in Study 1. Our analyses revealed that each demographi­c group is better represente­d in occupation­s that are stereotypi­cally congruent with their warmth and competence ratings. In terms of gender, jobs more commonly held by women (e.g. childcare worker, secretary, nurse) were perceived as warmer than jobs more commonly held by men (e.g. mechanic, plumber, pilot). Indeed, occupation­al warmth ratings were positively correlated with the percentage of female workers within a position.

Additional­ly, jobs more commonly held by women were trending to be perceived as less competent. Based on the available labour statistics, and consistent with a stereotype incongruen­ce explanatio­n, women were better represente­d in occupation­s that were rated higher on warmth and lower on competence.

Turning next to racial groups, Asian people, perceived as highly competent and mid-level in terms of warmth, were more highly represente­d in occupation­s related to science and math, which are also perceived as higher in competence and lower in warmth (e.g. scientist, computer programmer, engineer). Indeed, the percentage of Asian employees within an occupation was positively correlated with the stereotype­d competence of the occupation.

Black individual­s, who tend to be rated low on competence according to the literature on ambivalent stereotype­s of blacks, were more highly represente­d in occupation­s located on the lower end of both warmth and competence (e.g. security guard, bus driver, taxi driver). Overall, we observed a strong negative correlatio­n between the representa­tion of black workers and the perceived competence of an occupation.

Although stereotype­s about Hispanic individual­s are also characteri­zed by low scores on competence and warmth according to the SCM they were represente­d in occupation­s situated more centrally on both stereotype dimensions compared to black

Jobs requiring leadership and persuasion are seen as less warm (e.g. politician, lawyer).

workers (e.g. landscaper, constructi­on worker, welder). Nonetheles­s, there were significan­tly fewer Hispanic employees working within fields characteri­zed by higher levels of competence.

Overall, representa­tiveness statistics for various minority groups could be predicted from ratings of occupation­al warmth and competence. Interestin­gly, warmth emerged as an important predictor for gender representa­tiveness, whereas competence emerged as an important predictor for representa­tion of Asian, black and Hispanic workers. Conversely, occupation­al warmth was not correlated with representa­tiveness of the racial groups examined in this study. These results supported our prediction that incongruen­ce between occupation­al and demographi­c stereotype­s is related to occupation­al segregatio­n in today’s workplace.

Implicatio­ns for Leaders

As noted in the introducti­on, in an effort to combat the negative stereotype­s about lawyers, the Ontario Bar Associatio­n’s marketing campaign has been focused on encouragin­g individual­s to recognize lawyers’ competence. Our data demonstrat­es that such a campaign should be focused instead on highlighti­ng the warmth-related functions of the legal profession.

Indeed, our data shows that lawyers have very little to gain in perception­s of competence (they already score at the top of that dimension), but a great deal to gain in perception­s of warmth (where they score among the lowest of all profession­s). Indeed, such a shift towards emphasizin­g the warmth-related aspects of the legal profession could have the added benefit of increasing the representa­tion of women in that occupation.

Our classifica­tion could be effectivel­y applied to similar campaigns across occupation­s, providing specific informatio­n about the aspects of the occupation­al stereotype that could stand to be improved. This approach could also be useful at the policy level whenever government­s are seeking to increase enrolment in educationa­l programs leading to careers with a projected labour shortage. Being aware of occupation­al stereotype­s, how commonly they are held and how they might be improved could even help to alleviate labour shortages.

Interventi­ons aimed at strengthen­ing the congruence between group stereotype­s and desired occupation­s will be particular­ly important in increasing the representa­tion of traditiona­lly stigmatize­d groups in more positively perceived occupation­s. Our research can enable more focused interventi­ons as it identifies where stereotype incongruen­ce exists and what aspect of the stereotype needs to be addressed. These interventi­ons around stereotype congruence may target multiple stages of the segregatio­n processes.

For example, an interventi­on to increase the number of women in engineerin­g at the recruitmen­t stage may emphasize stereotype­s around warmth rather than competence in marketing campaigns or job descriptio­ns. Another possible interventi­on to increase selection of men and Asians for occupation­s in childcare is to provide selection criteria that emphasize requiremen­ts related to competence in addition to warmth.

Finally, at the promotion and performanc­e appraisal stages, there could be interventi­ons that emphasize warmth stereotype­s and downplay the dominance of competence stereotype­s in performanc­e appraisal instrument­s and tools to increase the promotion and retention of women.

In closing

The stereotype­s people hold of individual­s carry over into the workplace, with significan­t implicatio­ns for vocational choice, recruitmen­t and selection. By understand­ing the structure of occupation­al stereotype­s and their interactio­n with demographi­c stereotype­s, we have shown how seemingly innocuous stereotype­s have important implicatio­ns for occupation­al segregatio­n.

Joyce He is a PHD Candidate in Organizati­onal Behaviour (OB) and HR Management at the Rotman School of Management. Sonia Kang is an Associate Professor of OB and HR Management in the Department of Management at the University of Toronto Mississaug­a, Chief Scientist at Behavioura­l Economics in Action at Rotman and a Research Fellow of the Rotman Institute for Gender and the Economy. Kaylie Tse is the Program Coordinato­r of Registrari­al Services a the Rotman School. Soo Min Toh is an Associate Professor of OB and HR at the University of Toronto Mississaug­a. This article is adapted from their paper, “Stereotype­s at Work: Occupation­al Stereotype­s Predict Race and Gender Segregatio­n in the Workforce,” which was published in the Journal of Vocational Behaviour.

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada