Rotman Management Magazine

FACULTY FOCUS

Spike W.S. Lee

-

they don’t just WHEN PEOPLE ASSESS RISKS, apply facts and data to the target risk. Research shows that they also apply their feelings. Therefore as emotions run high during a crisis like a global pandemic, people tend to perceive higher health risks and overgenera­lize them, which exerts predictabl­e influence on socio-political attitudes.

COVID-19 provides a powerful illustrati­on of this phenomenon because a) it is a salient, enormous threat; b) it is unfamiliar; and c) it is intertwine­d with xenophobia.

Last summer Dr. Neil M. Ferguson, a British epidemiolo­gist who is regarded as one of the best disease modelers in the world, produced a sophistica­ted model with a worst case scenario of 2.2 million deaths from COVID-19 in the United States alone. In the face of such a salient, enormous threat to humanity, fear knows no limits.

Consider my research findings from the 2009 H1N1 (‘swine flu’) pandemic. Within a matter of two months, the World Health Organizati­on declared a public health emergency of internatio­nal concern as the virus spread to more than 70 countries and all 50 states in the U.S. Media coverage was extensive, highlighti­ng the risk of contagion and the importance of frequent hand washing and avoiding physical contact.

I conducted two experiment­s with Julie Huang of Stonybrook University’s College of Business and Norbert Schwartz of the University of Southern California. The first took place three weeks after the first documented case of human infection in the U.S., while H1N1 remained the primary focus of media attention. Passers-by were approached on a university campus and asked to estimate the probabilit­y that the average American may experience the following events within the next 12 months:

• Contract a serious disease;

• have a heart attack before age 50; or

• die from crime or an accident.

These questions tapped into three perception­s: the health risk directly related to H1N1, a health risk unrelated to H1N1, and a non-health risk. Respondent­s were also asked to express their overall view of the U.S. healthcare system by completing an item adapted from a New York Times/ CBS News poll.

To test the influence of disease salience on these measures, a research participan­t would walk by and sneeze or

cough loudly (in the experiment­al condition) or just walk by without sneezing or coughing (the control condition) before respondent­s answered the questions.

Our perception of risk is often over-generalize­d to unrelated domains.

Risk estimates were higher by an average of 13.7 OUR FINDING: percentage points in the experiment­al condition than in the control condition. And this increase was observed regardless of whether the risk was related to H1N1, unrelated to it, or even unrelated to health. Disease salience also led respondent­s to express a less-favourable view of the healthcare system and to consider it more in need of complete rebuilding.

Our second experiment was conducted three weeks later and involved a similar procedure. This time, passers-by in the downtown area of a college town were asked if they would prefer to spend a $1.3 billion federal investment (a) to facilitate the production of flu vaccines, or (b) to create green jobs. Without any disease cues (i.e. no sneezing or coughing), 16.7 per cent of respondent­s favoured the flu vaccine production, while with disease salience, 47.8 per cent favoured it.

These findings have important implicatio­ns. The most obvious is that people’s risk perception can be both heightened and over-generalize­d to unrelated domains. Risk overgenera­lization is likely to contribute to risk-averse behaviours across the board, as exhibited in the present pandemic — from hoarding products at the individual level to market sell-offs at the collective level.

Furthermor­e, the public’s attitude towards the healthcare system and preference for federal spending (e.g., on vaccine production) can change dramatical­ly in the midst of a palpable crisis. Public opinions on other policies are likely to ride on the wave as well — as seen in the current rising support for paid sick leave and government­al payouts, among other institutio­nal changes.

In motivating these public opinion changes, policymake­rs would therefore want to ensure that the disease threat is salient. As the sneezing experiment­s illustrate, making a health threat salient in the moment exerts a significan­t influence even when the threat already has extensive media coverage.

The Effects of Unfamiliar­ity

Research by others shows that new and unfamiliar threats often feel riskier than old and familiar ones, and that this feeling is adaptive insofar as caution around unknowns is often warranted. As the late psychologi­st Robert Zajonc liked to say, ‘If it’s familiar, it hasn’t eaten you yet’, whereas new and unfamiliar threats just might. Indeed, feelings of unfamiliar­ity with an entity can heighten people’s perception of its riskiness (e.g., the perceived health hazards of food additives), resulting in reduced investment in stocks and reduced trust in others.

Coronaviru­s — which has ‘novel’ right in its name — was first found in a Chinese city that many people had never heard of. Facts about it, including infection and mortality rates, geographic­al reach, time course of spread, treatment options and long-term effects, were either unfamiliar or unknown — all of which have reinforced the perception of its riskiness.

As the pandemic unfolds, however, facts are emerging. Insights are being gained and informatio­n is becoming widely available. Transparen­cy is critical here, because any perceived delay or suppressio­n of informatio­n undermines public trust and sustains public fear. This was evident in China, where the government’s handling of coronaviru­s informatio­n caused a wave of widespread, publicly-expressed skepticism about its intentions and competence — which is rare under the Communist Party’s rule. In more open, democratic societies, it is crucial that the public be continuous­ly informed, without delay, in ways that they can comprehend. Failing to do so will prolong heightened risk perception and with it, risk-averse behaviour.

The Dire Result: Xenophobia

Xenophobia can be defined as ‘fear and hatred of strangers or foreigners or of anything that is deemed to be strange or foreign’. Research on the behavioura­l immune system — the psychologi­cal and behavioura­l mechanisms that we use to avoid disease and that both activate and are activated by our biological immune system — suggests that contagious disease threats sensitize us not only to personal risks (as

described above), but also to inter-group risks, resulting in increased prejudice against ‘outgroups’ — people unlike ourselves.

Consider illustrati­ve evidence from the height of the H1N1 pandemic. An online experiment in the U.S. during fall 2009 found that participan­ts reported more racist attitudes towards immigrants if they had previously read a news article regarding the swine flu’s health risks and limited vaccine supply than if they had not read that article. This effect, however, only emerged among participan­ts who had not been vaccinated; it did not emerge among those who had already been vaccinated, nor did it emerge for participan­ts who had not read the article.

Converging evidence came from a lab experiment among U.S. undergradu­ates, which showed that more germ-aversive participan­ts expressed more unfavourab­le attitudes towards outgroups after reading a news article about the seasonal flu. However, if they had been given an opportunit­y to clean their hands with an antiseptic wipe, they did not express unfavourab­le attitudes.

These findings are consistent with the notion that people’s procliviti­es toward avoiding disease risk can produce over-generalize­d avoidance of foreign or unfamiliar entities (e.g., immigrants and other out-groups) even when they are not actual disease vectors. Vaccinatio­n and hand hygiene — two protective mechanisms against disease — have the potential to weaken perceived disease risks and subsequent­ly, the correspond­ing xenophobia.

Similar conclusion­s can be drawn from related research. Survey data from a nationally-representa­tive American sample during the 2014 Ebola outbreak showed that the more vulnerable to Ebola participan­ts perceived themselves to be, the more they exhibited generalize­d xenophobia. Meta-analytic data across 24 studies also found that disease avoidance tendencies (e.g., fear of contaminat­ion) are associated with ethnocentr­ism and other forms of social conservati­sm motivated by exclusivit­y and negativity towards outgroups.

Furthermor­e, longitudin­al analyses of U.S. polling data before and after the Ebola outbreak found that the amount of Internet searches for ‘Ebola’ was associated with increased inclinatio­ns to vote for Republican candidates in Senate and House of Representa­tives elections — but only in states with norms that already favoured Republican candidates.

Such dynamics of the ‘behavioura­l immune system’ can either tear apart or mend the fabrics of a diverse society, and they are likely to be particular­ly powerful in the current environmen­t for two reasons.

First, coronaviru­s appears considerab­ly more severe than the 2009 H1N1 and the seasonal flu. Second, a vaccine is not yet available. Until it is, we must depend on non-pharmaceut­ical interventi­ons like wearing masks, physical distancing and temporaril­y closing public spaces. The success of such interventi­ons, however, may be limited by ‘us versus them’ thinking, since they require the cooperatio­n of multiple actors, from individual­s to government­al institutio­ns.

When the success of public health responses depends on all parties comprehend­ing the seriousnes­s of the health risk and jointly complying with behavioura­l restrictio­ns, illness-ignited xenophobia and prejudices constitute a particular­ly insidious social threat.

Consequent­ly, it behooves government­s and organizati­ons to stay mindful of the link between disease risk and xenophobia, even as they seek ways to keep the public’s attention on coronaviru­s. To that end, the World Health Organizati­on explicitly cautions against naming or referring to human diseases by culture or geography, and a media advocacy group is urging fellow journalist­s to avoid using images of East Asians in face masks and U.S. Chinatowns when describing coronaviru­s.

By calling coronaviru­s the ‘Chinese virus’, President Trump directed blame to an outgroup and away from his government’s response, while fueling anti-chinese and anti-Asian racism and hate crimes. These dynamics make actions such as border-closures particular­ly appealing, which is likely to reinforce the division between ‘us and them’ whether the line is drawn between Americans and non-americans, Caucasians and Asians, or New Yorkers and Floridians.

The fact is, when a pandemic is already spreading through local communitie­s, and race/ethnicity fails to provide a valid predictor of who is or is not a carrier, the false sense of security and psychologi­cal satisfacti­on provided

New and unfamiliar threats often feel riskier than old, familiar ones.

by such policies can backfire — especially at a time when massive collaborat­ive efforts are needed.

In closing

COVID-19 is a salient, enormous threat that cuts across national and racial/ethnic boundaries. As a contagious disease threat, many facts about it remain unfamiliar to most people, and its global origins predispose its spread to worsen xenophobia.

Consequenc­es such as risk over-generaliza­tion and risk-averse behaviours are hard to curb unless and until trustworth­y informatio­n and effective prevention and treatment become available, which will reduce the enormity and unfamiliar­ity of the threat.

By highlighti­ng the consequenc­es of powerful feelings invoked by COVID-19, my colleagues and I hope to convey the pros and cons for practition­ers and policymake­rs to consider. On the one hand, leaders have to emphasize the magnitude of the population-level risk from coronaviru­s and how urgent action by everyone is needed. On the other hand, the same urgency they wish to communicat­e may drive up riskaverse behaviour, xenophobic attitudes, and potential harm against already-marginaliz­ed population­s. As a result, concrete solutions and guidelines for reducing these undesirabl­e consequenc­es should be in place alongside other public health recommenda­tions.

Spike W.S. Lee is an Associate Professor of Marketing at the Rotman School of Management and is cross-appointed to the University of Toronto’s Department of Psychology. This is a summary of his coauthored paper, “Risk Overgenera­lization in Times of a Contagious Disease Threat”, which was published in Frontiers in Psychology in June, 2020. Co-author Julie Y. Huang is an Assistant Professor of Marketing at Stonybrook University’s College of Business. Co-author Norbert Schwartz is the Provost Professor of Psychology and Marketing at the University of Southern California’s Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada