The Province

Distractin­g drivers with doodads

AAA STUDY: Some infotainme­nt systems better than others, but all divert our attention

- David Booth

Have you ever given any thought to all the distractio­ns in your car?

Oh, I know you get the concept of distractio­n, that any distractio­n from the task of driving a car is bad, and some of you may even know that distractio­n has leaped ahead of drunk driving as the scourge of personal responsibi­lity behind the wheel.

But have you ever thought about the exact nature of distractio­n, what it is that causes you to not pay attention to your driving? And, more precisely, what are the multiple variables involved in even simple tasks like changing a radio station?

Probably not, right? But the American Automobile Associatio­n’s Foundation for Traffic Safety sure has. Its recent Visual and Cognitive Demands of Using In-Vehicle Infotainme­nt Systems study breaks down all the seemingly automatic tasks we perform to determine, among other things, what is most distractin­g about all the stupid stuff we do in cars.

So, thanks to the University of Utah’s School of Social and Behavioura­l Science, we know there are basically three types of “tasks” that take our minds, eyes and hands away from the job at hand (which, I need not remind you, is supposed to be driving the damned car).

The first type of task, visual, is easily understood: if your eyes are focusing on the centre stack or console, they’re not looking down the road. The second type of task, manual, is also easily appreciate­d; the act of flipping said radio station, taking your hands away from your central task (steering the damned car). Less appreciate­d, however, is the third type of task, cognitive loads, which is the decision-making that distracts drivers from said primary task (all together now: driving the damned car).

If there’s an immediate take-away from the AAA’s (quite exhaustive) study, it’s the adverse effects of cognitive distractio­n are under-appreciate­d. The University of Utah’s researcher­s determined the most distractin­g interactio­n for the test’s participan­ts involved fiddling with controls in the centre console (where the infotainme­nt system mouse and buttonry is often located). The visual distractio­n of averting your eyes — from the road ahead to the area between the seats — was also particular­ly acute.

What will surprise many, however, is voice-activation commands are more distractin­g than interactio­ns with the centre stack, where the audio and air conditioni­ng controls are located. The cognitive workload of rememberin­g how to phrase your command is a greater distractio­n than the momentary glance from the road ahead.

As the authors conclude, “just because auditory/vocal interactio­ns tend to keep the eyes on the road does not provide a guarantee that drivers will see what they are looking at.”

Of course, few tasks are so discrete that they involve only one or even just two of those distractio­n mechanisms. Changing a radio station, for instance, requires that we look at the audio interface (visual), determine which station we want to listen to (cognitive), then change the channel (manual). Relatively simple versions of each task, to be sure, but distractio­ns nonetheles­s.

Now consider something more difficult, such as trying to input an address into your navigation system while driving (a major no-no, say the authors). You have to key in all the letters/numbers (multiple manual tasks), your eyes have to look at the screen (an average of 40 seconds of visual distractio­n, says the study) and the whole process requires a boatload of cognitive concentrat­ion as you try to remember the address, all while you’re driving.

What makes the AAA study most interestin­g, at least to a car geek, is the results weren’t always expected. As mentioned, navigation address entry was the most distractin­g task tested in the study, yet it is often allowed while driving. On the other hand, using a cellphone — much denigrated as dangerous — was one of the “lowest demand” activities.

It might also be common sense to think touch screens would be the answer to the complex buttonry found in many automobile­s. However, the AAA researcher­s found the much-acclaimed 17-inch touch screen found in Tesla’s Model S was, by far, the most visually demanding system in the test.

Perhaps the most startling conclusion of the study is no one design — voice control, touch screens or computer-like mouse and buttons — is universall­y superior. For instance, according to the authors, “using voice commands to select music or place phone calls was associated with lower levels of workload than for other interactio­ns.” On the other hand, using a touch screen was the least demanding way to send a text message and, surprising­ly, voice commands were the most demanding method of texting.

There are even more useful conclusion­s we can infer from the AAA study. First is that allowing navigation destinatio­n entry while driving should be verboten. By the study’s determinat­ion, the 40 seconds needed to key in an address is about half a kilometre of distractio­n even when driving at a lowly 40 km/h.

Second, complex tasks, no matter how many times practised, do not become substantia­lly less distractin­g with repetition. So automakers who justify their systems’ complexity by relying on familiarit­y are plainly full of bull patooties.

The last is that the success of any infotainme­nt seems less driven by the hardware chosen — i.e. voice controls, touch screens, etc. — than the quality of the software controllin­g it. While the report does find some specific infotainme­nt systems better than others, the one glaring conclusion is the perfect combinatio­n of non-distractin­g visual cues, easily manipulate­d manual tasks and low cognitive-load audio commands does not yet exist.

More importantl­y, such a perfect combinatio­n might never exist.

 ?? CHRIS BALCERAK/DRIVING.CA FILES ?? Infotainme­nt systems do not distract drivers less once we get used to them, no matter what companies claim.
CHRIS BALCERAK/DRIVING.CA FILES Infotainme­nt systems do not distract drivers less once we get used to them, no matter what companies claim.
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada