Toronto Star

Want to read minds? Read good books

Fifty Shades of Grey is fun, but new study prefers War and Peace

- KELLY SERVICK

Fifty Shades of Grey may be a fun read, but it’s not going to help you probe the minds of others the way War and Peace might. That’s the conclusion of a new study, which finds that, compared with mainstream fiction, highbrow literary works do more to improve our ability to understand the thoughts, emotions and motivation­s of those around us.

Perhaps it’s no surprise that the lead author of the new study, David Kidd, came to social psychology by way of Russian literature. Now a Ph.D. student at the New School in New York City, he is versed in arguments from literary theorists that divide fiction into two categories.

When we read a thrilling-but-predictabl­e bestseller, “the text sort of grabs us and takes us on a roller-coaster ride,” he says, “and we all sort of experience the same thing.” Literature, on the other hand, gives the reader a lot more responsibi­lity. Its imaginary worlds are full of characters with confusing or unexplaine­d motivation­s. There are no reliable instructio­ns about whom to trust or how to feel.

Kidd and his adviser, social psychologi­st Emanuele Castano, suspected that the skills we use to navigate these ambiguous fictional worlds serve us well in real life. In particular, the duo surmised that they enhance our so-called theory of mind. That’s the ability to intuit someone else’s mental state — to know, for example, that when someone raises their hand toward us, they’re trying to give us a high-five rather than slap us. It’s closely related to empathy, the ability to recognize and share the feelings of others. Increasing evidence supports the relationsh­ip between reading fiction and theory of mind. But much of this evidence is based on correlatio­ns: Self-reported avid readers or those familiar with fiction also tend to perform better on certain tests of empathy, for example.

To measure the immediate cognitive effects of two types of fiction, Castano and Kidd designed five related experiment­s. In each, they asked subjects to read 10 to 15 pages of either literary or popular writing. Literary excerpts included short stories by Anton Chekhov and Don DeLillo, as well as recent winners of the PEN/O. Henry Prize and the National Book Award. For more “mainstream” selections, they looked to top-sellers such as Danielle Steel’s The Sins of the Mother and Gillian Flynn’s Gone Girl, and to anthologie­s of genre fiction, including a scifi story by Robert Heinlein.

When participan­ts finished their excerpts, they took tests designed to measure theory of mind. In one test, the Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy 2-Adult Faces (DANVA2-AF) test, they looked at a face for two seconds and decided whether the person appeared happy, angry, afraid or sad. In the more nuanced Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), they saw only a small slice of a face and picked from four complex emotions such as “contemplat­ive” and “skeptical.”

On average, both groups did slightly better on these tests than control subjects who read either a nonfiction article or nothing at all. This fits with previous research showing a positive relationsh­ip between fiction and theory of mind. But among the fiction readers, those who read “literary” works scored significan­tly higher on the theory of mind tests than those who read popular selections, Kidd and Castano report online in Science. The absolute difference­s in scores were hardly dramatic: On average, the literary group outperform­ed the popular group by about two questions (out of 36) on the RMET test, and missed one fewer question (out of 18) on the DANVA2-AF. But psychologi­st Raymond Mar of York University, notes that even very small effects could be meaningful, provided they translate into real-world consequenc­es-reducing the likelihood that social misunderst­andings could create grudges or leave someone in tears.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada