Toronto Star

FAMILY TIES

Researcher­s are tracing the ‘common ancestors’ of popular folk tales, just as biologists understand evolution by comparing species’ DNA

- SARAH KAPLAN

By studying the ‘DNA’ of popular fairy tales, researcher­s look for the origins of modern cultures and languages,

Jacob and Wilhelm Grimm weren’t trying to get famous writing stories for kids.

The brothers were trained philologis­ts, serious young men who grew up in poverty and wanted to make a name for themselves doing something important. They had a cause — German nationalis­m — and a mission to uncover the origins of the German language. If they spent a lot of time reading fairy tales, it was because they believed those stories revealed something fundamenta­l about German language and culture.

They aimed to trace the evolution of their nation — which, in the mid-19th century, was still so fragmented and in flux that it barely warranted the title — via stories about talking animals, clever children, evil stepmother­s and tricksters of all sorts.

So they searched old book collection­s, chatted with friends, sought out some peasant mothers with a few good yarns up their sleeves. And they found something a lot bigger, and older, than the Germans.

According to Sara Graca da Silva and Jamshid Tehrani, authors of a study published Jan. 20 in the journal Royal Society Open Science, many of the fairy tales we associate with the Grimm Brothers, Hans Christian Andersen and Disney are thousands of years older than the people who first stuck them in a book. Some of them are so old they predate modern languages and reli- gions — one is even older than writing itself.

Rather than being unique to certain cultures — “Snow White” as essentiall­y German, for example — these stories evolved from common ancestors, much the same way living things did. And in the same way biologists understand evolution by comparing animals’ DNA, da Silva and Tehrani say they can elucidate mysteries about the origins of cultures by looking at the stories they tell.

Not all of this is new. For more than a century, folklorist­s have been grouping tales from disparate parts of the world according to shared themes, many of which are charmingly (and perhaps disturbing­ly) specific: “The Obstinate Wife Learns to Obey,” for example, or “The Lecherous Holy Man and the Maiden in a Box.”

The Aarne-Thompson-Uther (ATU) classifica­tion system, the gold standard for such groupings, includes more than 2,000 kinds of stories that can be applied to fables from all over the world. Clearly, the tales we use to tease, terrify and lull our kids to sleep share some common cultural DNA.

Indeed, the Brothers Grimm themselves wouldn’t have been surprised by da Silva and Tehrani’s finding. Way back in 1884, Wilhelm Grimm asserted that people who spoke languages that shared Indo-European ancestry — the idea of an Indo-European language family had recently become mainstream — might also share folklore, and that the contents of his Children’s and

Household Tales weren’t simply German but also part of a much broader tradition.

Yet many folklorist­s dispute this notion. If disparate cultures share stories, they argue, it’s because they’ve been passed through societies by trade, conquest, migration and war. You can’t chart two tales back to a common ancestor because there’s too much cross-contaminat­ion.

“The consensus was that these processes would have destroyed any deep signatures of descent from ancient ancestral population­s,” Tehrani explained to the Atlantic.

And even if stories do bear traces of their ancient origins, theories about their evolution are difficult to prove.

Before the printing press, folk tales were only transmitte­d orally — no medieval monk was going to spend decades illuminati­ng a manuscript about Rapunzel and her unwieldy hair. And however influentia­l they might be, stories don’t leave much in the way of a fossil record. If “Little Red Riding Hood” existed several thousand years ago, the story didn’t leave any physical proof of its presence until someone thought to write it down.

Which is why da Silva and Tehrani approached the task of tracing stories like geneticist­s, rather than fossil hunters. If a researcher can figure out the relationsh­ips between species by scanning their DNA and pinpointin­g their last common ancestor, why can’t folklorist­s do the same?

According to Tehrani, an anthropolo­gist at the University of Durham in the U.K., they can.

“Folk tales . . . evolve through similar processes as biological species (variation, selection and inheritanc­e),” he wrote for the Conversati­on in 2013. “. . . Phylogenet­ics can fill (gaps in the literary record) by using informatio­n about the past that has been preserved through the mechanism of inheritanc­e.”

He published his first phylogenet­ic analysis (in biology, phylogenet­ics is the study of evolutiona­ry history and relationsh­ips among organisms) of a folk tale in the journal PLOS One back in 2013, using “Little Red Riding Hood” (type No. 333 in the ATU classifica­tion system) as his guinea pig.

The story exists in countless forms across the European, Asian and African continents. There’s the one most North American readers know, about a little girl in a red cloak who gets eaten by a wolf dressed as her grandmothe­r and has to be cut out of his stomach by a friendly huntsman.

But there’s also a version from East Asia in which a leopard disguised as a grandmothe­r convinces a group of sisters to let him into the house, then eats one of the girls before the others escape. Then there’s the story from central Africa of a girl who is tricked by an ogre pretending to be her brother, gets eaten, and is only released when her brother tracks the ogre down and kills the impostor. All of them share traits with another type of story known as “The Wolf and the Kids” (ATU 123) in which a group of goat kids are devoured by a wolf who tricks them into thinking it's their mother.

By analyzing the language, characters and plots from these tales - the stories' “genes,” so to speak — Tehrani constructe­d a family tree of the type you might see in an exhibit at a natural history museum. Starting with a single shared ancestor that arose somewhere between Europe and the Mid-

dle East roughly 2,000 years ago, the stories branched off into the groups that would become ATU 333 and ATU 123. The 333 family line would give rise to the familiar Grimm version of Red Riding Hood, while the African version is actually more closely related to the 123 family. The East Asian version, he concluded, is a 333 relative that borrowed some traits from its 123 cousins.

The research offered an interestin­g look at how stories evolve, but Tehrani argued that its impact is more than just academic.

“Folk tales, more than any other type of story, embody our shared fantasies, fears and experience­s,” he wrote in 2013. “Understand­ing which elements of them remain stable and which ones change as they get transmitte­d across generation­s and societies can therefore provide a unique window into universal and variable aspects of the human condition.”

Tehrani tackled his next project with that goal in mind. Along with da Silva, who studies intersecti­ons between evolution and literature at the New University of Lisbon in Portugal, he pieced together phylogenet­ic trees for 275 story types from the “Tales of Magic” category in the ATU system.

In 76 cases, the duo was able to trace the story back hundreds or even thousands of years. The oldest of them — a tale about a blacksmith who sells his soul to the devil in exchange for unmatched smithing powers, and then uses those powers to pin the devil down, allowing the smith to keep both his soul and his awesome new abilities — dates back 6,000 years to the very beginning of the Bronze Age.

If true, this finding may clear up some confusion about the origins of the Proto-Indo-European language speakers who first started telling that story. Very little is known about the people who launched the language family that would come to encompass everything from Sanskrit to Latin and Urdu to English. As Mark Damen, a historian at Utah State University, explains it:

“There is still no unequivoca­l evidence from either historical or archeologi­cal sources for exactly where, when or how the original speakers of Proto-Indo-European lived. No site, no technology, no extant historical text, no particular past event has as yet been definitive­ly associated with the people whose descendant­s would later spread Indo-European culture and language across the entire globe. The Indo-Europeans are at present in strictest terms a linguistic phenomenon, which is not to say their culture never existed — there is overwhelmi­ng evidence it must have at some point in history and, without doubt, somewhere in Eurasia, but that’s not very precise.”

Still, there are generally two schools of thought about where a huge swath of the world’s cultural origins might have been. One proposes that Proto-Indo-European language speakers were Neolithic farmers living 9,000 years ago in what is now Turkey, while the second argues that they were pastoralis­ts from the Russian steppes who knew how to work metal.

If it’s true that “The Smith and the Devil” (ATU 330) — a story now told in a broad swath of Indo-European languages — really does date back 6,000 years, it could be a big boon for the latter school of thought. The 9,000-year-old Turks lived before the invention of metallurgy, and were unlikely to have told a story whose hero was a blacksmith. Those Russian pastoralis­ts, on the other hand, fit the bill perfectly. Reconstruc­ted versions of the Indo-European vocabulary include a possible word for metal, according to da Silva and Tehrani’s study, and the fact that these people lived at the very beginning of the Bronze Age “suggests a plausible context for the cultural evolution of a tale about a cunning smith who attains a superhuman level of mastery over his craft.”

But this detail was also a sticking point with some other researcher­s who read the study.

John Lindow, a folklorist at the University of California at Berkeley, told Science News that the Proto-Indo-European vocabulary for working with metal was fairly limited. It’s not clear that the term “smith” even existed, he argued, which casts doubt on the claim that “The Smith and the Devil” is as old and significan­t as Tehrani and da Silva say.

But Tehrani rebutted that argument. And, speaking to the Atlantic, he was already envisionin­g future research into why some tales are told for thousands of years, and what plot elements or motifs seem to persist through the various retellings.

“We think this is the start of a much bigger project using oral traditions and storytelli­ng as windows into the lives of our ancestors,” he said.

 ??  ?? According to a new study, some of the fairy tales associated with the Grimm Brothers, Hans Christian Andersen and Disney are thousands of years older than their first written examples, predating modern languages and religions.
According to a new study, some of the fairy tales associated with the Grimm Brothers, Hans Christian Andersen and Disney are thousands of years older than their first written examples, predating modern languages and religions.
 ??  ??
 ?? PETER MOUNTAIN/DISNEY ENTERPRISE­S ??
PETER MOUNTAIN/DISNEY ENTERPRISE­S

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada