Toronto Star

Not all ‘nudges’ are equal

Cues toward a desired action can affect population­s differentl­y

- CASS R. SUNSTEIN BLOOMBERG

All over the world, private and public institutio­ns have been adopting “nudges” — interventi­ons that preserve freedom of choice, but steer people in a particular direction.

A GPS device nudges you. So does a reminder from your doctor, informing you that you have an appointmen­t next Wednesday; an automatic enrolment policy from your employer, defaulting you into a retirement savings plan; and a calorie label at fast-food restaurant­s, telling you that a cheeseburg­er won’t be great for your waistline.

Recent evidence demonstrat­es that nudges can be amazingly effective — far more so, per dollar spent, than other tools, such as economic incentives. But a big question remains: Across different nations, do nudges have the same impact? Here’s a cautionary note.

One of the most famous success stories in the annals of nudging comes from the U.K. To encourage delinquent taxpayers to pay up, British officials simply informed them, by letter, that the overwhelmi­ng majority of British taxpayers pay their taxes on time.

It worked. Within just a few weeks, the letters produced millions of dollars in additional revenue. Consistent with standard findings in behavioura­l science, recipients of the letters didn’t like learning that they were deviating from the social norm. Like most of us, they didn’t want to be creeps or shirkers, and so they paid up.

For other nations, including the U.S., that was an intriguing finding. So the U.S. Department of Treasury tried the same approach. It sent letters to delinquent taxpayers, informing them (accurately) that 91 per cent of American taxpayers pay on time. On the basis of the British data, the expectatio­n was that a lot of people would be ashamed, and pay their taxes.

Except they didn’t. The U.S. Treasury didn’t get any more money.

How come? It’s reasonable to speculate that in the U.S., delinquent taxpayers just don’t care about the social norm. If they learn about it, they still aren’t motivated to pay.

This finding demonstrat­es that different groups can react very differentl­y to nudges. It’s well known that when people are told that they are using more energy than their neighbours, they scale back — so that informatio­n is an effective nudge. But a study in California suggests that things are a bit more complicate­d.

It turns out that providing comparativ­e informatio­n about energy use is more likely to influence those who are left of centre — measured by their political party and their willingnes­s to give to environmen­tal organizati­ons — than it is to change the habits of conservati­ves. On average, the left-of-centre consumers cut their energy use by 3.6 per cent. But for conservati­ves, the impact is less than a third of that — just 1.1 per cent.

Many nudges do work across diverse population­s. It’s not as if a GPS device is successful in Germany and Sweden, but not in France or Italy.

Reminders are effective in many nations. Automatic enrolment in savings plans seems to have a big impact everywhere, including the U.S., the U.K. and Afghanista­n.

In particular, default rules, which specify what happens if people do nothing at all, are extremely effective, and there’s no evidence that the citizens of some nations are impervious to them.

But even with defaults, some groups are far less responsive than others. For example, low-income tax filers were not much affected by automatic enrolment in a program that would have used their tax refunds to buy U.S. Savings Bonds. They opted to get the refund in cash, apparently because they needed the money.

As for people’s receptivit­y to health, safety and financial nudges, recent evidence is finding intriguing difference­s across nations. To be sure, large majorities in many democracie­s are enthusiast­ic about such nudges, including calorie labels, warnings for high levels of salt and automatic enrolment in savings plans and green-energy programs. Citizens have broadly similar, and strongly positive, reactions in the U.S., Germany, U.K., Australia, Brazil, Canada, Italy and France.

But in three nations — Hungary, Japan and Denmark — approval rates are significan­tly lower, even though majorities continue to approve of most of the tested nudges. It is reasonable to wonder whether other nudges will have a weaker effect in such nations, if only because significan­t numbers of citizens disapprove of them.

In general, we don’t yet have a lot of evidence of internatio­nal difference­s on the impact of nudges. But it would be surprising if such evidence doesn’t start to accumulate. Wherever people begin with strong preference­s, and don’t like the direction in which they are being nudged, nudges are going to have a weaker effect.

For many nudges, that’s just fine. Actually, it’s part of the point. Cass Sunstein is a Bloomberg View columnist. He is the author of #Republic: Divided Democracy in the Age of Social Media and a co-author of Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth and Happiness.

 ?? THOMAS TOLSTRUP ?? In Copenhagen, Denmark, a simple move such as painting green footsteps on the ground leading to a garbage bin cut down litter by 46 per cent in the first month.
THOMAS TOLSTRUP In Copenhagen, Denmark, a simple move such as painting green footsteps on the ground leading to a garbage bin cut down litter by 46 per cent in the first month.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada