Toronto Star

Understand­ing the universal symbol of soccer shame,

Holding your head in your hands may be a form of hugging yourself after a crucial mistake

- DAVID GENDELMAN

Goals in soccer games can be few and far between, which helps explain the delirious nature of most scoring celebratio­ns. Some players yank off their jerseys or drop to their knees and glide across the turf in glee. They all often end up at the bottom of a pile of jubilant teammates.

Then there are the players who are presented with a goal-scoring opportunit­y and, for whatever reason, fail. When this happens, they all do the same thing: raise their hands and place them on their heads — apparently the universal gesture to signify, “Oh my goodness, how did I miss that?”

If you’ve followed the World Cup this summer, you’ve most likely seen it dozens of times.

Lionel Messi has done it, and so has Cristiano Ronaldo. Players from France and Croatia, who meet Sunday in the final, have also struck the disappoint­ed pose. It has nothing to do with soccer and everything to do with the human psyche, according to zoologists, psychologi­sts and others who study such things.

“It’s going to tell others, ‘I get it and I’m sorry, therefore you don’t have to kick me out of the group.’ ” JESSICA TRACY PSYCHOLOGY PROFESSOR

The gesture signifies that “you know you messed up,” said Jessica Tracy, a professor of psychology at the University of British Columbia. “It’s going to tell others, ‘I get it and I’m sorry, therefore you don’t have to kick me out of the group, you don’t have to kill me.’ ”

It’s not limited to the shooter, either. In one of the most replayed soccer blunders of all time, from the 2010 World Cup, Yakubu Aiyegbeni of Nigeria missed an empty net from about three metres away. Though Aiyegbeni hardly moved afterward, nearly every one of his teammates and coaches made the gesture in immediate and unrehearse­d synchroniz­ation.

In his seminal 1981 study of the sport, The Soccer Tribe, zoologist Desmond Morris included the gesture in his catalogue of 12 player reactions to defeat. He noted its function of self-comfort, which he described as “a form of auto-contact, a widespread device used when the individual feels in need of a reassuring embrace, but has no one immediatel­y available to offer one.” It’s seen among non-human primates as well.

In 2008, Tracy published an influentia­l study with her colleague David Matsumoto in which they studied the gestures of both victory and defeat made by sighted and congenital­ly blind Olympic athletes. They found evidence to suggest that the display behaviours of pride and shame were innate and universal.

“You have the head in the hands — that’s shame,” Tracy said. “You have the constricti­on of the body, in the way that the player is moving his arms around his head, almost to make himself smaller. Those are very classical shame display elements.”

No one knows better than the players when they mess up. Cobi Jones, who had a long career with the U.S. men’s national team and now works as a TV analyst, said in a telephone interview that a blatant miss triggers, along with the gesture, a sense of disbelief and embarrassm­ent. “That’s what we train for, day in and day out, to put that ball in the net,” he said. “And that’s a simple one. It’s one that you shouldn’t miss.”

The gesture is also commonly made after a goalkeeper makes a spectacula­r save to prevent what otherwise would be a sure goal. One of the most famous examples came in the 2006 World Cup final. In extra time of a tie game, French star Zinedine Zidane snapped a header that he thought was destined to win the game, only to see the Italian goalkeeper, Gianluigi Buffon, nudge the ball over the crossbar with his fingertips. Zidane’s hands went straight to the top of his head.

Whether the ball misses the net because of a gaffe by the shooter or a spectacula­r save by the goalkeeper, the response by the rejected players remains nearly identical. “It’s exactly the same statistica­l reality,” British soccer historian David Goldblatt said. “You have your chance. You miss it, a goalkeeper saves it, whatever. The mechanism by which you get to that point is neither here nor there.”

Jones described the offensive player’s experience in both cases as “shock.”

“When people get startled unexpected­ly, their hand will sort of move up to their head almost in a protective motion,” said Dacher Keltner, a professor of psychology at the University of California, Berkeley. “The oldest kind of behavioura­l intention in that class of behaviours is to protect your head from blows.”

In 1996, Keltner published a study of people’s emotional reactions to sudden blasts of noise. The subjects of that study had reaction similar to that of soccer players after a near miss. “You hear a really loud sound and that’s in the world correlativ­e that something might hit your head, and you defend your head, which is vulnerable and critical,” Keltner said. “Any kind of action like the near miss, the source of psychic pain will produce those head protective movements.”

The basic gesture might be accompanie­d by subtle add-ons. Players might cover their faces with their hands or jersey, another typical shame display. Or they might tilt their head upward to the sky in what Goldblatt sees as “asking for the miss to be interprete­d as a consequenc­e of the fates rather than their own mistake.”

“When people feel awestruck, they look up,” said Keltner, who has spent time with the Golden State Warriors’ coaching staff discussing his work on compassion. Something he observed from that experience was that “serious athletes recognize chance more so than fans.” The look to the sky, he said, could be the player’s “recognitio­n of something beyond human agency.”

The hands-to-the-head gesture is also performed by fans at the same moments as the players. Since they are observers rather than participan­ts, their motivation­s could differ. Philip Furley, a lecturer in sports psychology at the German Sports University, in Cologne, has studied players’ behaviour during penalty kicks, when the gesture is common.

Among spectators, Furley said, “what you often find is this sort of contagion going on. If it’s a team you support, then if this player you’re supporting in this moment shows something you might be infected by his non-verbal behaviour.”

No matter the cause, the predictabi­lity of the gesture has become its most defining trait. “It’s like punch lines, catchphras­es that comedians use,” Goldblatt said. “People start laughing before you say them. A lot of comics work on that.”

In this case, soccer players and fans don’t need to work on the gesture. It seems to come naturally.

 ?? JEWEL SAMAD/AFP/GETTY IMAGES ?? Brazil's brilliant Neymar shows his suffering as his side falls to Belgium in their World Cup quarter-final.
JEWEL SAMAD/AFP/GETTY IMAGES Brazil's brilliant Neymar shows his suffering as his side falls to Belgium in their World Cup quarter-final.
 ?? ELSA/GETTY IMAGES ?? Goalie Wilfredo Caballero of Argentina after his mistake led to a goal for Croatia.
ELSA/GETTY IMAGES Goalie Wilfredo Caballero of Argentina after his mistake led to a goal for Croatia.
 ?? LUIS ACOSTA/AFP/GETTY IMAGES ?? German players lift their hands to their heads in unison during a game against South Korea. Failures and mistakes on the soccer pitch prompt a near-uniform response.
LUIS ACOSTA/AFP/GETTY IMAGES German players lift their hands to their heads in unison during a game against South Korea. Failures and mistakes on the soccer pitch prompt a near-uniform response.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada