Toronto Star

If you could breed their minds …

In fact, we can — study finds selective breeding has changed dogs’ brains

- JASON BITTEL

Dogs are a weird bunch. They range in size from bearish behemoths like Newfoundla­nds to pipsqueaks like Milly, a Chihuahua who measures less than four inches tall and holds the Guinness record for World’s Shortest Dog.

Some breeds are fast runners. Others are jumpers, swimmers or diggers. Bloodhound­s specialize in sniffing, while greyhounds hunt primarily by sight. Border collies excel at herding, Jack Russell terriers at flushing foxes from dens.

Over at least 15,000 years, and especially since a Victorian-era dog-creation craze, selective breeding by humans has resulted in a single species with more physical variation than almost any other in the animal kingdom. And now, scientists have provided the first evidence that all of this selective tweaking hasn’t just changed dogs’ sizes, shapes, colours and behaviours — it’s also altered the way their brains are built.

Their research, published Monday in the journal JNeurosci, began with MRI scans from 62 dogs that had visited the University of Georgia Veterinary Teaching Hospital for neurologic­al evaluation­s.

All the dogs, representi­ng 33 breeds, were discharged with clean bills of brain health. But their scans provided the scientists with a trove of data.

“The first question we wanted to ask was, are the brains of different breeds of dogs different?” said Erin Hecht, a neuroscien­tist studying dog cognition at Harvard University and lead author of the study.

Indeed, from dachshunds to Dobermans, the scientists found well-defined difference­s between dog brains, even after accounting for things such as the pooches’ overall size and shape.

By looking at the areas of the dogs’ brains that varied most across the breeds, the scientists were able to create maps of six neural networks linked to certain functions, such as the sense of smell or movement. The team found the shape of these networks “correlated significan­tly” with common traits associated with each breed, as described by the American Kennel Club. “Brain anatomy varies across dog breeds,” Hecht said, “and it appears that at least some of this variation is due to selective breeding for particular behaviours like hunting, herding and guarding.”

In other words, not only do the shapes and sizes of canine brains vary by breed, the structures within those brains also are different. This discovery helps explain what makes a Maltese act like a Maltese, or a boxer like a boxer.

Jeffrey Stevens, director of the Canine Cognition and Human Interactio­n Lab at the University of Nebraska at Lincoln, called the study’s use of existing MRI data “clever” and its premise “exciting.” However, he offered some words of caution.

“The one thing that I think there’s a bit of disagreeme­nt on in the literature and in people’s views is how useful it is to map behaviours to breeds,” Stevens said. “There’s often a lot of variation within a breed, across individual­s.”

Stevens also noted the MRI scans weren’t performed as the dogs were performing breedspeci­fic tasks, making it difficult to draw big conclusion­s linking breed to behaviour.

“This is very well known in the human neuroimagi­ng field, where you want to be really careful drawing any inferences about cognitive processes that are based on brain activity that you’re not directly testing,” Stevens said.

But this raises another intriguing question. Most dogs today do not actively fill the roles for which their breed was created. In fact, all 63 study dogs were house pets, not working dogs. So even though they may be the descendant­s of great herders or hunters, they probably don’t perform those tasks in any serious capacity. That could make a big difference.

“It’s not like (your brain) gets a new wrinkle every time you learn something,” Hecht said. “But there have been lots of studies that show your brain changes as you learn a new language or as you learn a new motor skill.”

So it’s quite possible that a Labrador retriever that does the job its kind was bred to do — retrieving birds shot by hunters — might have a brain that looks different from a Lab that retrieves popcorn stuck between couch cushions.

Stevens said he viewed this as a hint that the researcher­s might be onto something. If they’ve managed to find such significan­t variation in pets, he said, imagine what might be discovered in the brains of working dogs.

“The correlatio­ns actually could be stronger if you used animals that were still bred for those purposes,” Stevens said.

Daniel Horschler, a PhD student at the University of Arizona’s Canine Cognition Center, said the variation found across dog breeds could prove to be an important model for understand­ing how brains work in general. Previous studies have investigat­ed neurologic­al difference­s between species, Horschler said, but such animals “obviously have a lot more difference­s in terms of their ecology and their behaviour and their environmen­t, so being able to do this in a single species is really interestin­g.”

Hecht said her team is trying to better understand why and to what extent variations within a breed occur — or, as she put it, the difference­s between “high-skill performers and lowskill performers.”

“For example, border collies who are winning herding competitio­ns out in the real world and siblings of those dogs who, for whatever reason, would rather just sit on the couch,” she said.

 ?? MATT CARDY GETTY IMAGES FILE PHOTO ?? Scientists found well-defined difference­s between the brains of different dog breeds, even after accounting for things such as the dogs’ overall size and shape.
MATT CARDY GETTY IMAGES FILE PHOTO Scientists found well-defined difference­s between the brains of different dog breeds, even after accounting for things such as the dogs’ overall size and shape.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada