The Hindu (Kolkata)

The legacy of ‘Moneyball’: objective metrics and statistica­l analyses

The foundation­al research of Kahneman and Tversky demonstrat­ed how the human mind consistent­ly made mistakes when it was required to make decisions in unpredicta­ble circumstan­ces. Their research gave rise to the subject of behavioura­l economics

- Atanu Biswas REUTERS

A scene from Moneyball. ichael Lewis’s groundbrea­king 2003 book Moneyball, which was later adapted into a Brad Pittstarre­r movie in Hollywood, is one of the significan­t publicatio­ns that defined the first quarter of the twentyfirs­t century. Billy Beane, the book’s primary protagonis­t and manager of the Oakland Athletics (Oakland A’s) in U.S.’s wealthy Major League Baseball (MLB), was a former MLB player who went into the personnel side of the business and had remarkable success there in spite of a tight budget. For recruiting players, he utilised better “metrics,” such as “onbase percentage,” which gauges how often a batter reaches base and is now regarded as being superior to more convention­al metrics, such as batting average, to assess offensive skill. He did this by using historical data and analytics. Under Billy Beane, Oakland A’s discovered value in players who had been passed over or ignored, and folly in much of what passed for baseball wisdom. Furthermor­e, the influentia­l 2003 book Moneyball caused its perceived significan­ce to soar.

And ever since, it has also started to propel civilisati­on forward.

MThe lapse in analysing data

From a wider angle, the book sets up the rationale behind the (seemingly irrational) craziness around data in today’s world. Well, if a novel analytical method had produced new insights in baseball, could it not produce similar results in any other area of human endeavour? To make judgements in every area of their lives, people today strive to churn massive amounts of data. “Moneyballi­sation” is everywhere, in various areas of government, business, education, sports, farming, election campaign, banking, the investigat­ion of criminal activity, and so on.

However, in August 2003, a review of the book appeared in The New Republic, written by Richard Thaler, an economist, and Cass Sunstein, a law professor, then both at the University of Chicago. They thought it was intriguing that a poor team like the Oakland A’s could beat most teams by taking advantage of the inefficien­cies in any market for profession­al sports. They continued, however, by claiming that the author of Moneyball appeared to be unaware of the deeper cause of the inadequaci­es in the baseball player market, which originated from the fundamenta­l principles of human nature. Israeli psychologi­sts Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky had long ago detailed the ways in which certain baseball specialist­s might misunderst­and baseball players — that is, the ways in which any expert’s judgements might be distorted by their own thoughts. Therefore, the book Moneyball was only an example of concepts that had been discussed for many years. “It didn’t take me long to figure out that, in a not so roundabout way, Kahneman and Tversky had made my baseball story possible,” Lewis subsequent­ly wrote in Vanity Fair.

“Why do profession­al baseball executives, many of whom have spent their lives in the game, make so many colossal mistakes?” wrote Thaler and Sunstein in their review. Lewis’s book simply made the observatio­n that when baseball experts evaluated players, their judgement could be clouded by their prejudices and preconcept­ions. However, why?

Why our minds act unpredicta­bly

Actually, Kahneman and Tversky had written a number of oddball papers between 1971 and 1984 exploring the ways in which human judgement could be distorted when we are making decisions in the face of uncertaint­y. In a 1982 study titled “Judgement under Uncertaint­y: Heuristics and Biases,” they found that when evaluating the likelihood of a particular outcome, people frequently depend on readily recalled informatio­n rather than actual data — a concept known as the “availabili­ty heuristic.”

Subsequent­ly, in a 2004 paper published in the Michigan Law Review, Thaler and Sunstein argued that Lewis’s clues can be explained by several studies in cognitive psychology. “Yet reliable statistica­l evidence will outperform the availabili­ty heuristic every time. In using data rather than profession­al intuitions, Beane confirmed this point.” As it happens, Beane’s preference for performanc­e scouting over prototypeb­ased scouting aligns well with a 1999 work by Kahneman and S. Frederick on the representa­tiveness heuristic, which highlighte­d the applicabil­ity of dualproces­s theories of cognition. The cognitive operations are divided into two families. System I operates quickly, automatica­lly, and with ease; it’s intuitive. System II is reflective, slower, selfaware, calculativ­e, and deductive in comparison. According to Kahneman and Frederick, System I provides rapid solutions to problems of judgement, whereas System II acts as a monitor, validating or overriding those judgements. Traditiona­l scouts employ System I and emphasise the “mind’s eye.” Based on data analytics, Beane insisted on a System II override.

‘A cognitive minefield’

Overall, the foundation­al research of Kahneman and Tversky demonstrat­ed how the human mind consistent­ly made mistakes when it was required to make decisions in unpredicta­ble circumstan­ces.

Their research transforme­d even Big Data studies and gave rise to the subject of behavioura­l economics, which is the synthesis of psychology and economics. Beane’s endeavour just constitute­d an early exercise in this regard.

“Recognise the signs that you are a cognitive minefield.” This is how Kahneman described his research for lay audiences in his 2011 book Thinking, Fast and Slow. Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Prize in economics in 2002, which many feel Tversky would have shared if he hadn’t died in 1996. Nassim Taleb, the author of The Black Swan, reportedly remarked that Kahneman might be the only Nobel laureate in economics whose results can be replicated!

“No area of baseball is more susceptibl­e to bias than scouting, in which organisati­ons aggregate informatio­n from disparate sources: statistica­l models, subjective evaluation­s, characteri­sations of mental makeup and more,” Joe Lemire wrote in a 2021 article for The New York Times. “Kahneman emphasised the importance of maintainin­g independen­ce of judgments to decorrelat­e errors — that is, to separate inputs so that one doesn’t influence another,” Lemire added. Kahneman’s theory may very well be applicable in similar situations, such as recruiting players for the lucrative Indian Premier League.

Daniel Kahneman, the economics unicorn who altered the field forever, died on March 27 at the age of 90. The intriguing partnershi­p between Tversky and Kahneman was discussed in detail by Michael Lewis in his 2016 book The Undoing Project – A Friendship That Changed Our Minds. It’s possible that these two avantgarde psychologi­sts altered humanity’s perception of its own mind in the process.

The writer is Professor of Statistics, Indian Statistica­l Institute, Kolkata.

 ?? ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from India