Irish Independent

Don’t be so quick to swat fruit flies as you could hurt their feelings

Evolving ideas of animal consciousn­ess pose difficult questions

- ED CUMMING

Elephants mourn their dead, octopuses play catch, grey parrots recognise themselves in the mirror. But how about crabs that weigh up decisions, bees that play games and fruit flies that dream?

The last decade has seen rapid developmen­ts in the field of animal consciousn­ess, upending widely held assumption­s about which sorts of animals exhibit signs of consciousn­ess.

The New York Declaratio­n on Animal Consciousn­ess, signed last week by 39 leading scientists and academics, aims to reframe the conversati­on, bringing surprising new species into the mix.

“Striking new results have hinted at surprising­ly rich inner lives in a very wide range of other animals,” the declaratio­n says, “including many invertebra­tes, driving renewed debate about animal consciousn­ess.”

It asserts that as well as “strong scientific support for attributio­ns of conscious experience to other mammals and birds”, there is also a “realistic possibilit­y of consciousn­ess in all vertebrate­s and many invertebra­tes”.

Finally, the declaratio­n says that “when there is a realistic possibilit­y of conscious experience in an animal, it is irresponsi­ble to ignore that possibilit­y in decisions affecting that animal”.

The declaratio­n has been co-organised by Jonathan Birch, an associate professor in the department of philosophy at the London School of Economics and author of a book, The Edge Of Sentience: Risk And Precaution In Humans,

OtherAnima­lsAndAI, due to be released later this year.

It follows the Cambridge Declaratio­n on Consciousn­ess in 2012, which recognised possible consciousn­ess in cephalopod­s, birds and non-human mammals, and reflects a fast-changing field.

“We want to create a moment to get people to notice this emerging science of animal consciousn­ess,” Prof Birch says.

“It’s a topic that has long been marginalis­ed in science. It’s only in the last 10 to 15 years that there has been a broadening of the ambitions of consciousn­ess science to also study the experience of other animals.

“The evidence has come in from a wide range of animals indeed that it’s not just mammals, or vertebrate­s like birds and reptiles and fish, but also invertebra­tes − octopuses, crabs, lobsters and insects.

“In evolutiona­ry terms, the inferences get a little bit more tentative, and the evidence perhaps a little weaker, but we think even in insects there’s a realistic possibilit­y of conscious experience­s.”

For many laypeople, the most persuasive example of non-mammal intelligen­ce are octopuses, whose remarkable behaviours have been made famous by many articles and documentar­ies – most notably the Oscar-winning film My Octopus Teacher, in which a South African filmmaker forms a friendship with a common octopus who plays with him, and shows remarkable ingenuity in learning to fight off sharks.

“During my training in the early 2000s, animal sentience and consciousn­ess were treated like pink elephants, conspicuou­sly ignored in serious scientific discourse,” says Dr Alexandra Schnell, a leading octopus researcher and documentar­y maker. “However, the burgeoning field of animal sentience has revitalise­d these topics, allowing discussion­s about subjective experience­s in animals to flourish.”

We now know octopuses can use tools, solve mazes and recognise human faces.

“Updating the declaratio­n of consciousn­ess to include more animals, such as cephalopod­s, is pivotal because it recognises their complex cognitive and emotional capacities, ensuring more ethical and informed treatment,” she adds.

“This acknowledg­ment can lead to better protection under animal-welfare laws, more humane handling in research and food industries, and a broader understand­ing of consciousn­ess across different species.”

They aren’t the only sea creatures surprising researcher­s.

For Professor Bob Elwood, of Queen’s University Belfast, the starting point for his research was a chance encounter with the chef Rick Stein. When Prof

Elwood explained his specialism in animal welfare, Stein asked him if lobsters could feel pain.

“I’d studied crustacean­s for a long time but it had never crossed my mind,” he says. “The opinion at the time was that they responded by a nociceptiv­e reflex − we have those also − if you touch something hot you move your hand rapidly, before the message gets to the brain.

“The pain kicks in after you move your hand. These reflexes are important. If crustacean­s simply responded by reflex there would be no worry about them feeling pain when you were boiling them or dismemberi­ng them alive.”

He decided that while he could not prove they were feeling pain, in a human sense, he could try to find out if they were responding just by reflex.

His results were remarkable. He found that crabs will rapidly learn to avoid a dark shelter in which they receive electric shocks and use another which is safe, and they will do it in just two trials. Crayfish will do it within one trial. “It’s really rapid and it’s not just a reflex.”

In another experiment, he put caustic soda or acetic acid on prawn antennae.

“They would rub the affected antenna against the side of the tank and rub it through their little claws,” he says. “And you could reduce it by applying a local anaestheti­c.

“No one of these experiment­s is critical in suggesting consciousn­ess or pain. But when you see again and again, with different studies asking different questions, and they start agreeing, and you get evidence supporting the idea of pain, you cannot dismiss it. You have to take it into account. The more studies are done, the more likely it is that these animals experience pain.

“We now think they should be given the benefit of the doubt. They shouldn’t be boiled or torn up when they’re alive.”

For Bruno van Swinderen, a professor at the University of Queensland, the humble fruit fly offers a way to examine the origins of consciousn­ess mechanisti­cally. “I am at the bottom of the pecking order of conscious animals,” he says. “You’ve got cats and dogs, dolphins and elephants. Even bees do interestin­g cognitive things. Then there’s fruit flies, which I work on. Why would they be included in this?

“But it’s worth trying to understand from the bottom up why consciousn­ess may have evolved, why it is adaptive. If you understand it that way, it makes more sense to study the fruit fly.”

Part of the reason Prof van Swinderen has signed this declaratio­n, and one of the reasons the field is enjoying such a surge of interest, is the discussion around the possibilit­y of conscious artificial intelligen­ce.

“It’s a very opportune time,” he says. “We’re seeing all these conversati­ons about AI, about when machines will get conscious. From a human perspectiv­e it’s hard to understand, but from a bottom-up perspectiv­e it’s easier to understand how consciousn­ess might evolve from a simple circuit, like a fly brain circuit.”

If we are prepared to entertain the prospect of consciousn­ess in a computer, we must be prepared to entertain it in fruit flies, too: a remarkable but chastening thought. (© Telegraph Media Group Ltd 2024)

“They should get the benefit of the doubt. They shouldn’t be boiled or torn up when they’re alive”

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Ireland