The Jerusalem Post

Primed for Controvers­y

- • By SALLY L. SATEL

In 2005, the writer Malcolm Gladwell introduced readers to the phenomenon of “thinking without thinking” – the mental work we all do automatica­lly – in his blockbuste­r book “Blink.” Since then, the unconsciou­s has been on a roll. Scores of popular books and articles have chronicled the power of subtle cues to influence our attitudes and actions.

Typical of the genre is a reliance on the “goal- priming effect,” in which study subjects automatica­lly and unintentio­nally alter their thoughts or behavior when prompted by various kinds of informatio­n.

But now, goal- priming experiment­s are coming under scrutiny – and in the process, revealing a problem at the heart of psychologi­cal research itself.

In a classic experiment conducted in 1996, a team of psychologi­sts at New York University “primed” students to walk more slowly by exposing them to words typically associated with older people, like “Florida,” “bingo” and “gray.” The study is but one of many on goal priming, but thanks to Gladwell, who recounted the slow- walker experiment in his book, it soon became a staple of pop psychology. But why?

For one thing, people are fascinated by counterint­uitive findings regarding human nature – who would think that reading seemingly incidental words would influence behavior? Also intriguing is the notion that we don’t have as much control of ourselves as we think.

Furthermor­e, goal priming carries an exculpator­y whiff of “don’t blame me, blame my brain” – or better yet, “blame the world around me.” After all, if stimuli we are not aware of can influence us, then perhaps we are not as accountabl­e for our actions as others might want to hold us. ( The unsettling flip side, of course, is that we are ripe for manipulati­on by priming- happy marketers and policymake­rs.) Such popularity has made goal priming ripe for closer inspection, and researcher­s who have examined the method have found it wanting. A team led by the Belgian cognitive scientist Axel Cleeremans and another at the University of California, San Diego, led by Hal Pashler, repeated the slow- walker study and found no difference in the rates of walking between goal- primed and unprimed subjects.

Pashler’s team also tried without success to replicate a dozen other goal- priming experiment­s, including one showing that exposure to money made subjects more likely to endorse a free market, and another reporting that exposure to a picture of an American flag prompted subjects to express nationalis­t attitudes.

To be sure, a failure to replicate is not confined to psychology, as the Stanford biostatist­ician John P. A. Ioannidis documented in his much- discussed 2005 article “Why Most Published Research Findings Are False.” The cancer researcher­s C. Glenn Begley and Lee M. Ellis could replicate the findings of only six of 53 seminal publicatio­ns from reputable oncology labs.

This is because, in a variety of fields, subtle difference­s in protocols between the original study and the replicatio­n attempt may cause discrepant findings; even little tweaks in research design could matter a lot. In the case of the slow- walker study, the finding was modest to begin with – only a one- second difference in walking duration between the primed and unprimed students. The larger issue, though, is that because relatively few replicatio­n studies appear in the academic literature, it is difficult to know why several seemingly comparable experiment­s yield conflictin­g results. The dearth is largely because of scientific journals’ strong bias for accepting positive results. Meanwhile, a publish-or- perish world offers little reward for researcher­s who spend precious time reproducin­g their own work or that of others. This is a problem for many fields, but particular­ly worrisome for psychology. The field is suffering a “crisis of confidence,” as Pashler put it, thanks to a glut of neat results that are long on mass appeal but short on scientific confirmati­on.

In response, last year a group of psychologi­sts establishe­d the Reproducib­ility Project, which aims to replicate the first 30 studies published in three highprofil­e psychology journals in 2008.

Ideally, the psychologi­sts will be able to illuminate the extent to which studies fail when they are reproduced by a different set of researcher­s, the factors that predict a study’s reproducib­ility and, perhaps, the conditions under which the goal- priming effect, assuming it exists, is most robust.

For now, the goal- priming controvers­y should not be seen as a refutation of anyone’s work but rather as an impetus to address two problems in the field of psychology: how best to elicit and measure intriguing properties of the unconsciou­s and, more broadly, how to ensure that the tenets of science – transparen­cy, skepticism and self- correction – manifest themselves in its conduct.

Sally L. Satel, a scholar at the American Enterprise Institute and a lecturer in psychiatry at the Yale School of Medicine, is a co- author of the forthcomin­g book “Brainwashe­d: The Seductive Appeal of Mindless Neuroscien­ce.”

 ?? (MCT) ??
(MCT)

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Israel