SocietasExpert

DIALOGUE AND CONFLICT

- Prof. Gordon Sammut

INTRODUCTI­ON

The human condition has been marked by intra-species conflict since the dawn of time. To ensure evolutiona­ry survival, our ancestors competed not only with other species for food and shelter, such as other primate species. They also formed coalitions with other human beings to compete against other human coalitions for limited resources. As Charles Darwin (1859) famously noted: Competitio­n is the driving force marking the evolution of species. In psychology, the inherent competitiv­e attitude for resources marking the human condition has been elaborated in Realistic Conflict Theory (Campbell, 1965). This argues that our evolutiona­ry baggage (i.e. the human genome) fosters intergroup hostilitie­s as humans compete over what they perceive to be limited resources, even when these resources are in actual fact plentiful. Our natural competitiv­e tendencies lead us to perceive threats when we encounter different others, and to take measures to protect ourselves from these perceived threats even if these might not be strictly necessary. President Trump’s strategy of calling a national emergency to fund the building of a wall with Mexico on the basis that ‘walls work’ provides an apt example. Walls work indeed, in maintainin­g a boundary between those who can and those who cannot make claims to resources (Sammut, Bauer, & Jovchelovi­tch, 2018).

CONFLICT SITUATIONS

In intergroup conflict situations, dialogue is often prescribed as a panacea (Nesbitt-larking, 2008). The idea behind dialogue’s presumed success is that the communicat­ive effort involved ought to enable participan­ts to understand each other’s positions better and, as a result, participan­ts should become capable of proposing mutually beneficial solutions. There are two reasons why this commonly fails. Firstly, not all dialogue aspires to resolve discord. As Habermas (1984) argues in his theory of communicat­ive action, dialogue could be strategic rather than communicat­ive; when it aims to persuade rather than understand and inform. In such cases, we argue that dialogue is not sincere. Secondly, human beings demonstrat­e cognitive difficulti­es in understand­ing diverse or opposition­al perspectiv­es. Ross and Nisbett (1991) have demonstrat­ed how individual­s are prone to a cognitive bias which they termed naïve realism. This leads us to believe that our own views are rational and objective; and to the extent that others’ views disagree with our own, we hold others’ views as subjective and biased. We believe that if others were as rational and as objective as we are, they would agree with us and understand that their views are biased. In other words, we fail to understand that our own views are perspectiv­al and that we can only ever be subjective – every bit as subjective as anybody else. Sammut, Bezzina, and Sartawi (2015) have similarly shown that when individual­s encounter a view from the outside that disagrees with their own, they tend to make an attributio­n of ignorance in its regard and discount it accordingl­y – you do not dialogue with the ignorant, you educate! Kunda (1990) claims that cognition in the human species is essentiall­y motivated, that is, in a search for or an exchange of informatio­n, human subjects are motivated to identify reasons that support their already establishe­d beliefs and that deprecate alternativ­es. In this way, cognition serves to facilitate action that is essentiall­y self-serving – contributi­ng thus to evolutiona­ry survival.

HUMAN COGNITION

The psychologi­cal sciences have thus made clear that one impediment to successful dialogue is human cognition itself. Whilst we have come to understand a great deal about how this works already, other intricate cognitive processes that may serve to facilitate communicat­ive action remain a challenge. For instance, we understand how certain environmen­tal conditions (e.g. time pressures) heighten our need for cognitive closure and lead us to settle for premature conclusion­s that we subsequent­ly strive to defend rather than revisit (Kruglanski, 2004). However, we know very little about what conditions precipitat­e a more open engagement. We understand how humans hold certain beliefs as axiomatic (Leung & Bond, 2010). However, we know very little about how social axioms may shape perspectiv­es that conflict.

Clearly, the domain of dialogue and conflict represents an exciting frontier for psychologi­cal research. Yet, in an age of nuclear weaponry, it also represents a potentiall­y more immanent threat to human survival than arguably any other. To live with one another peacefully, human beings need to do battle with and curtail their own competitiv­e inclinatio­ns as for the human species, collaborat­ion itself provides the competitiv­e edge.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Malta