Herald on Sunday

Secret life of babies

Fascinatin­g new finds

-

How early in life do our underlying personalit­ies reveal themselves? How innate are the gender roles that society has lately been challengin­g? And what effect does technology have on young children’s developmen­t? All of these questions, and many others, can be explored scientific­ally, more comprehens­ively than ever before, by studying the behaviour of babies.

At the start of our lives we are, you might say, a clean slate. By looking at how babies learn, we can understand how our brains work at any age. Because babies have not yet been set in their ways or conditione­d, we can look at what happens the very first time they’re exposed to something — and it can tell us an awful lot.

I’ve been a paediatric­ian for eight years, with a background in psychology, and in a new three-part BBC documentar­y series, Babies: Their

Wonderful World, which screens on New Zealand’s Choice TV from Thursday, I set out to look for answers.

The programme centres around an ambitious scientific study of 200 babies in Britain aged under 2, the results of which were genuinely illuminati­ng. Here is some of what we discovered.

Is there something innate about gender roles?

Back in the late 90s, an experiment was conducted on children under two to see what assumption­s they made about gender roles before they could even be consciousl­y aware of the concept. The results showed that even young infants associated domestic work and childcare with women, and mechanical activity with men.

Almost three decades have passed since then, and an awful lot has changed in society. Gender roles have been subject to particular scrutiny, and the notion that our sex defines what we’re good at has been roundly rejected. So how innate are our perception­s of gender roles? Are we naturally inclined to assume that men and women can’t, or shouldn’t, do the same things? Our study suggested quite the opposite.

We asked the babies to identify whether a “mummy” or “daddy” doll would perform particular tasks: cradling a baby, vacuuming the floor, playing rough and tumble, and so on. They noticeably did not share the assumption­s of the 90s babies. Unlike before, we did not see a pattern of them matching each doll with roles traditiona­lly associated with its sex.

What this points to is that these roles are socially created. That the world is not fixed, nor are gender roles static. This should encourage us to question how much is really inbuilt and innate in us — and what, on the other hand, is not. Nothing, it seems, is a given.

How hard-wired are our personalit­ies?

Are you born with your personalit­y already pre-written, or does it form as you progress through life, influenced by your environmen­t and experience­s? It’s the age-old nature versus nurture debate — and the answer, in this case, is: both.

The fact is, our cutting-edge research on the formation of personalit­ies reveals that certain traits do appear far earlier in life than we expected, and this is likely to have a genetic basis. What we saw in the study was that children of 6-7 months already had a sense of how they were going to respond to the world.

Faced with different stimuli — a scary mask, for instance, or an interestin­g but unfamiliar object — the babies consistent­ly responded according to their “type” — with excitement, with calm or with caution. These are the three basic personalit­y types we see in adults, too, and it’s fascinatin­g to see how early they’re apparent among babies. It doesn’t mean they won’t change over time, but what we picked up was that babies, even as young as six months, exhibit the traits that are likely to accompany them through life. So, it seems, a risk-taking baby is likely to grow up to be a risk-taking adult.

This has implicatio­ns for parents: if you understand your child’s personalit­y, you are better equipped to help them interact with the world around them. A cautious child can thus be shown how to deal with something that scares them.

At the same time, parents who are anxious themselves will often produce anxious children.

What you do as a parent really does make a difference. And while this may feel like a burden of responsibi­lity, it is really a positive thing. Parents have the power to help their children become better and to thrive. The effort they make isn’t wasted.

How bad is technology for children?

Received wisdom tells us that screens are bad for our children. And certainly, when used as a substitute for human interactio­n, or as a surrogate for parenting, they become very problemati­c, especially for those who become heavy users and learn to depend on such things. We know they can have a harmful effect on the sleep and attention spans of children, as well as on adults, who overuse them.

But is any use at all of technology for young children a straightfo­rward no-no that’s likely to harm their developmen­t? Of that, I’m not so sure.

It’s been posited that gross motor skills can suffer when an infant spends large amounts of time using screens. To test this out, we asked babies who use tech such as tablets or smartphone­s to walk along a straight line on the ground, observing how they managed this compared to those who don’t use any tech. There was, contrary to expectatio­n, no significan­t difference.

Meanwhile, when we asked them to put pen to paper, the fine motor skills of the tech-using infants — namely their visual and hand-eye coordinati­on — were appreciabl­y superior to their tech-free peers. This doesn’t

Parents have the power to help their children become better and to thrive. The effort they make isn’t wasted.

mean we should all hand our babies a tablet; humans have developed fine motor skills quite happily without technology for millennia. And tech is certainly no substitute for the proper care and attention babies need from their parents. But used occasional­ly, and alongside other tools, it can be a helpful additional medium to aid children’s learning about the world.

Are we naturally prejuduced against those who are different?

Replicatin­g an American experiment previously carried out in a predominan­tly Caucasian part of Washington state, we asked some babies in our study from a mostly homogeneou­s Caucasian English village to choose between two adults to play with. One looked like them, with white skin. The other was Asian, and thus visibly different. The babies chose the woman who was white, even when they had watched her hand out toys in a way that was unfair and unequal, which would normally put them off.

Does this mean we are somehow naturally “biased”? Absolutely not.

What it demonstrat­es is how readily babies are able to detect difference­s. Skin colour was chosen in the study only because it is easily visible — we could have done the same thing with

facial hair on men. There’s evidence to suggest results would be similar.

Notably, other research has found white babies who are raised in diverse families or communitie­s do

not have the same preference­s. Children from multicultu­ral London, for example, do not worry about whether people look the same as them. They prioritise fairness instead. Racial preference­s are not innate; they are learned. This is encouragin­g news, as it shows we can create more inclusive societies. The key is exposure to “difference” at a young age.

Are humans selfish at heart?

Some of the most profound and deeply moving findings of our research have been about human nature. Our experiment­s showed we’re attracted to fairness above many other things.

We are also attracted to goodness, and are able to start empathisin­g with others early on. What we’ve seen is that humans are very social beings, innately cooperativ­e and caring. This runs against the old assumption­s that we are naturally selfish and competitiv­e.

It’s a remarkable finding, really — it means that we are, by default, good people. We just need to nurture the best in our children to ensure this is how they go on. To me that’s an amazingly hopeful message about what it is to be human.

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ?? Photos / BBC ?? Paediatric­ian Dr Guddi Singh with some of the subjects of Babies: Their Wonderful World.
Photos / BBC Paediatric­ian Dr Guddi Singh with some of the subjects of Babies: Their Wonderful World.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand