Otago Daily Times

Intriguing entangleme­nt of music and memories

Kelly Jakubowski explains why we are obsessed with music from our youth.

- Kelly Jakubowski is an assistant professor in music psychology at Durham University.

PEOPLE tend to be extremely nostalgic about the music they listened to when they were young. If you were a teenager in the 1970s, chances are you will love Queen, Stevie Wonder or Abba. And if you were young in the 1990s, Wannabe

by the Spice Girls probably still gets you on the dance floor.

But why is that? Do we genuinely think music from the past is better, or has it got something to do with the memories we have of that time?

Our recent study, published in

Music and Science, has come up with an intriguing answer.

Music is closely linked with memory and emotion. There is a reason for the popularity of the longrunnin­g BBC radio programme Desert Island Discs,

in which celebrity guests share the soundtrack of their lives. Or why the recent video of a retired ballerina with Alzheimer’s disease being spontaneou­sly brought back to her past through music went viral.

Music seems to be particular­ly associated with positive emotional memories with social themes, making it relevant for helping to improve life satisfacti­on during the pandemic.

General psychologi­cal research has shown that autobiogra­phical memories (life experience­s) from certain time periods are remembered better than others. One particular­ly notable phenomenon is the “reminiscen­ce bump”: the fact that people tend to disproport­ionately recall memories from when they were 10 to 30 years old.

Several theoretica­l explanatio­ns have been offered for this phenomenon, including that this lifetime period contains many novel and selfdefini­ng experience­s — which may be encoded in the brain more deeply and retrieved more easily. Biological and hormonal changes may also boost the effectiven­ess of our memories during this period.

It has been shown that when people are asked to choose their favourite record it is likely to come from the reminiscen­ce bump period, and that older adults know more about music from their youth than current pop songs. But does that mean that music from this period is more likely to be connected to autobiogra­phical memories?

In our study, my colleagues and I investigat­ed the presence of the “musical reminiscen­ce bump” in a group of 470 adults who were between 18 and 82 years old. Our aim was to investigat­e how a person’s age when a song was popular affected three related but distinct concepts: the degree to which the song was associated with autobiogra­phical memories, how familiar the song was and how much they liked the song.

Participan­ts in our study were shown the titles and artists of 111 pop songs that had featured in the charts across a 65year period (19502015) and provided ratings of the three concepts of interest.

We discovered that, across our participan­t sample as a whole, music that was in the charts during one’s adolescenc­e was not only rated as more familiar, but was also associated with more autobiogra­phical memories. This musicrelat­ed reminiscen­ce bump peaked around age 14: songs popular when participan­ts were this age evoked the most memories overall.

In addition, older adults (around age 40+) also liked songs from their adolescenc­e more than other songs. However, younger adults (aged 1840) did not show this same trend, and in some cases gave even lower liking ratings to music from their adolescenc­e than music released before they were born.

This suggests that songs from our adolescenc­e can become closely entangled with memories from our past even if we do not personally value the music. This may be because it has accompanie­d various memorable settings from this period (school dances, gatherings with friends, graduation­s, and so on).

Some songs were preferred regardless of a participan­t’s age when they were in the charts, however. For instance, we saw a general increase in how much people liked songs from the late 1970s to early 1980s, even in participan­ts who were not yet born during that time period.

This suggests pop music from certain time periods is intergener­ationally valued. Examples of songs we used from this time period include Hotel California by the Eagles, I Will Survive by Gloria Gaynor and Billie Jean by Michael Jackson.

So it seems that we are not primarily so interested in the music of our youth because we think it is better than music from other eras, but because it is closely linked to our personal memories. However, some songs may be able to transcend generation­al boundaries.

Advertiser­s who want to elicit a nostalgic reaction from a certain consumer demographi­c should take note. So should clinicians aiming to reconnect patients with selfdefini­ng memories from their pasts. — theconvers­ation.com

❛ It has been shown that when people are asked to choose their favourite record it is likely to come from the reminiscen­ce bump period, and that older adults know more about music from their youth than current pop songs

 ?? PHOTO: GETTY IMAGES ?? Never too old . . . Music is closely linked with memory and emotion.
PHOTO: GETTY IMAGES Never too old . . . Music is closely linked with memory and emotion.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand