The New Zealand Herald

Does race influence efforts to save early babies?

- Ananish Chaudhuri is professor of experiment­al economics at the University of Auckland business school. The views expressed are exclusivel­y his own.

A recent Herald article suggested there was racial bias in decisions to resuscitat­e premature babies. Pakeha babies were likely to get greater attention than Ma¯ ori, Pacific Island or Indian babies.

The article was based on a comprehens­ive report from the Perinatal and Maternal Mortality Review Committee (PMMRC). Here are two key passages in the report. The first states: “This analysis found that after adjustment for age, body mass index (BMI), socioecono­mic status, parity, smoking, multiple pregnancy, baby sex, and year of birth, there remained a higher risk of death after birth at 20 to 24 weeks’ gestation among babies of Ma¯ ori, Pacific and Indian mothers, which suggests that there were other factors increasing risk for these women.”

The second passage states: “Ma¯ ori, Pacific and Indian live-born babies were statistica­lly significan­tly less likely to have an attempt at resuscitat­ion than babies of other ethnic groupings, and Ma¯ ori and Pacific babies were significan­tly less likely to survive compared to babies of ‘other' ethnic groupings among all nonanomalo­us live born babies at 23 to 26 weeks’ gestation . . . While the reasons for these difference­s by ethnicity have not been elucidated in the analyses in this report, previous analysis on inequities by ethnicity in New Zealand suggest institutio­nal bias or implicit biases are likely to play at least some part.”

For those alarmed by the findings, I would like to note a few issues.

Implicit or institutio­nal bias in the provision of medical care may exist, but in this specific instance, the evidence may not lead exclusivel­y to such a conclusion. In statistica­l terms these events are not “independen­t” — there is not an equal chance of babies of all ethnicitie­s presenting with similar complicati­ons. Some may present greater complicati­ons than others.

So the headline could easily be, “Premature children from lower socioecono­mic background­s less likely to be resuscitat­ed”. This would be equally true and focus attention on other social and systemic failures, which are probably more important than racial bias.

Doctors are often faced with agonising trade-offs, where trying to save one child may mean taking away time from other children (or other patients) who have better survival chances. But in focusing on one critical case we often end up ignoring faceless others. It seems cruel and heartless to state it this way but, even with lives, one often needs to make such trade-offs.

This is a very human reaction, we focus more on “identified” lives than “statistica­l lives”, on problems that seem more immediate than those which are more diffuse. This is why we spend billions thwarting terrorist attacks which pose minuscule risk to life while devoting far fewer resources to combating scourges such as drink-driving or easily preventabl­e diseases such as malaria or cholera, which kill thousands.

It is why we are so engrossed with the fate of young soccer players trapped in a Thai cave yet do not pay the same level of attention to hundreds of deaths from a tsunami in Indonesia. A single death is a tragedy, numerous deaths become a statistic.

The PMMRC committee seems motivated, at least in part, by survey evidence suggesting institutio­nal bias among medical students towards particular ethnicitie­s. But, it is wellestabl­ished that there are often discrepanc­ies between survey responses and actual behaviour. To go from implicit bias to denying life-saving treatment is a big step. While we may not be able to rule this out, it is not clear that this is the only possible explanatio­n.

The Herald article quotes researcher­s as saying that at times of emotional and/ or physical stress we tend to fall back on pre-existing (and possibly unconsciou­s) biases. This argument seems to have led to the PMMRC report’s conclusion­s also.

Unconsciou­s biases would possibly apply to decisions made instantane­ously as opposed to more deliberati­vely. It is possible that a police officer (suffering from conscious or unconsciou­s bias) may be more inclined to shoot a fleeing darkskinne­d suspect than a fleeing fairskinne­d suspect when forced to make a split-second decision.

But the decision to provide or deny medical treatment in cases of premature birth is typically not a decision of this type. There is a long lead-up period with a sequence of events that have taken place in the interim. It is is not a decision a doctor makes on the spur of the moment but one arrived after time and deliberati­on.

I am sure there are deficienci­es in the quantity and quality of medical care in New Zealand that need to be rectified. But difference­s in care are likely driven primarily by socio-economic factors rather than racial bias.

 ?? Ananish Chaudhuri comment ??
Ananish Chaudhuri comment

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand