The Post

it's the little things

A growing number of researcher­s are focusing on short-term happiness, which they say matters more than your broader life contentmen­t, writes Luke Mintz.

-

In his final novel, Tender is the Night, F Scott Fitzgerald describes one summer’s afternoon in 1925, when his protagonis­t Rosemary, an American actress, is pottering around Amiens in northern France with her future boyfriend, toiling away the hours until dinner. “It was one of those uneventful times that seem at the moment only a link between past and future pleasure,” he writes, “but turn out to have been the pleasure itself ”.

It was an appreciati­on of those small, fleeting moments of fulfilment. We can all recall our own: a hand held by a loved one; the sound of rain on the roof as you drift off to sleep. Artists and musicians have long considered these joyful vignettes to be the bedrock of human happiness. Think of Lou Reed’s Perfect Day, consisting of nothing more than “sangria in the park” and “feed[ing] animals in the zoo”.

And now neuroscien­tists are starting to agree. The past few decades have seen an explosion of research into the so-called science of happiness, fuelled by advances in brain-scanning technology, plus a broader cultural shift towards seeing happiness as a key index of human developmen­t.

Traditiona­lly, happiness scientists have examined big-picture questions. Looking at your life in its totality, they might ask, are you broadly content? Do you find meaning in your job? Do you feel liked by your friends, and valued by your family? Debates can be controvers­ial, none more so than the question of money. In 1974, economist Richard Easterlin famously found that money does indeed buy happiness – but only up to a point, after which extra cash makes little difference. It became known as the Easterlin paradox.

Age is another major area of inquiry. Studies suggest that most of us move through life on a U-curve, in which happiness is high during our youth, drops in midlife, then rises again in our twilight years. Now a growing number of happiness researcher­s are ignoring these bigpicture questions. Instead, they are focusing on short-term cheeriness, or “mood”. How you feel from hour to hour matters, they argue, perhaps even more than your broader life “contentmen­t”, which is famously difficult to define or measure, and thus frustratin­gly tricky to do anything about.

Are you grumpy because somebody was rude to you on the train? Or are you upbeat because a friend compliment­ed your new dress? Economists have usually written off questions like these as unimportan­t “noise”, but the new generation of happiness scholars point to these small but significan­t moments as the bread and butter of happiness itself.

A key proponent of the new mood school is Robb Rutledge, honorary associate professor at University College London. In January, he launched app, The Happiness Project, which has been downloaded more than 15,000 times. It is the largest study of its kind, designed specifical­ly to identify the triggers of our short-term mood.

Users are asked to input demographi­c data about themselves. Then they play four games, themed around uncertaint­y, effort and learning – attributes thought to be crucial to short-term happiness. In one game, you play as a fisherman, deciding how much energy to give to each catch.

While playing, users are asked each 30 seconds to rank their happiness on a sliding scale. From tiny minute-by-minute movements, Rutledge hopes to collect vast amounts of data from which he can pinpoint the causes of high and low mood.

“Whether you feel good or bad right now is such a dominant feature of our conscious experience,” says Rutledge. “If you’re in a bad mood, you make different decisions. When I started this research, I found it surprising it wasn’t better understood. It changes pretty rapidly, and that’s actually ideal for doing detailed science.”

And last month, the project delivered its first results. Data from one of the games, which looks specifical­ly at the role of expectatio­ns, suggests that to optimise happiness you should lower your expectatio­ns slightly, but not so low for so long that it saps your motivation.

The project could give a particular­ly useful insight into mental illness. When diagnosed with clinical depression or anxiety, patients are asked about their mood, not their broader life fulfilment.

“People [with] specific patterns of happiness in the games might be better treated in certain ways. Some might be bothered when they have to press buttons faster to get points; others might be most bothered when they know they can’t win any points in the next section of the game.

“If we can better understand what makes happiness go up and down from minute to minute, then we can ask whether this also helps us predict and understand symptoms,” Rutledge says.

But the focus on minutiae is catching on. In recent years, some researcher­s have become interested in small moments of “awe”, defined by psychologi­st Professor Dacher Keltner as “the feeling of being in the presence of something vast and greater than the self ”. Think of a starry night in the countrysid­e, or catching a sunset from the perfect angle.

And in our social lives, too, small moments are getting big attention. We tend to think it is our close relationsh­ips that determine the lion’s share of our happiness, but research suggests that acquaintan­ces are more important than we realise.

In 2014, Dr Gillian Sandstrom, a psychology lecturer at the University of Essex, asked volunteers to record their social interactio­ns.

She found that participan­ts with large networks of loose “acquaintan­ces” were happier overall, and that participan­ts felt happier on days they recorded a higher number of casual interactio­ns.

As fickle as it seems, recording someone’s minute-by-minute mood might also provide a more methodolog­ically robust insight into their state of mind. If you ask a broad question such as “are you satisfied with your life?” it comes loaded with social pressure. Respondent­s are incentivis­ed to lie, even to themselves.

“They’re thinking, ‘What’s a good answer [my questioner] wouldn’t find weird’,” Rutledge says. “It is well noted that parents of young children are often unhappy because they are having to deal with something that is challengin­g. But they will usually say they’re satisfied.”

Does Rutledge worry he might create thousands of happiness-obsessives? “People don’t have to read what we discover,” he says, with a chuckle. But his findings could be fascinatin­g if we do choose to look at them, he thinks, providing an unpreceden­ted insight into our inner emotional terrain. And they might even cheer us up.

If you’ re in a bad mood , yo u make different decisions.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand