Weekend Herald

Weird Science

- With Herald science writer Jamie Morton: @jamienzher­ald Disgust was a basic emotion that

Why do we believe fake news?

Fake news is everywhere — but why we believe it is still unclear.

Psychologi­sts say valuing our identity more than our accuracy is what leads us to accept incorrect informatio­n that aligns with our chosen political party’s beliefs.

That value discrepanc­y, they believe, can explain why high-quality news sources are no longer enough — and understand­ing it can help us find better strategies to bridge the political divide.

It’s when our goals to fit in with certain groups are stronger than the goal we have to be accurate that we are more likely to be led astray,” says New York University psychologi­st Jay van Bavel.

This is what he calls his identity-based model of belief.

The idea is that we assign values to different ideas based on what matters to us most at the moment, and then compare those values to decide which idea we believe is true.

Because our political parties can provide us with a sense of belonging and help us define ourselves, agreeing with them can bolster our sense of self.

And that could sometimes matter more to us than accuracy about an issue.

Van Bavel believes his model offers strategies that can help bridge the political divide.

“Our model argues for increasing the value of truth or finding ways to reduce the effects of identity, whether on the left or the right.”

Van Bavel says we could increase the value of accurate beliefs by asking people to put their money where their mouth is.

“When you are in a disagreeme­nt, ask your opponent, ‘You wanna bet?’ And then their accuracy motives are increased, and you can see right away whether they are engaging in motivated reasoning,” he says.

We could also work to reduce the effects of identity by creating a “superordin­ate identity” — or getting people to think of themselves as citizens of a nation or the world rather than as members of a political party.

But we also have to pay attention to how we engage with people of different political persuasion­s.

“It turns out that if you insult them and publicly criticise them, their identity needs increase, and they become threatened and less concerned about accuracy,” he says.

“You actually need to affirm their identity before you present informatio­n that might be contradict­ory to what they believe.”

Can smell reveal our politics?

Thinking of Trump — does being more sickened by other people’s smells make you more likely to support him?

It’s an odd link to make, but researcher­s suggest that people who are easily disgusted by body odours are also drawn to authoritar­ian political leaders.

A survey showed a strong connection between supporting a society led by a despotic leader and being sensitive to body odours like sweat or urine.

It might come from a deep-seated instinct to avoid infectious diseases.

“There was a solid connection between how strongly someone was disgusted by smells and their desire to have a dictatorli­ke leader who can suppress radical protest movements and ensure that different groups ‘stay in their places’,” said study author Jonas Olofsson, of the University of Stockholm.

“That type of society reduces contact among different groups and, at least in theory, decreases the chance of becoming ill.” helped us survive. When people were disgusted, they wrinkled their noses and squinted their eyes, basically decreasing their sensory perception of the world.

At its core, disgust was a protection against things that are dangerous and infectious — things that we want to avoid.

The researcher­s had a theory that there would be a connection between feelings of disgust and how a person would want society to be organised.

They thought people with a strong instinct to distance themselves from unpleasant smells would also prefer a society where different groups were kept separate.

A scale was developed for the participan­ts to rate their levels of disgust for body odours, both their own and others.

This scale was used in a survey given online in different countries, together with questions on political views.

In the US, questions about how they planned to vote in the presidenti­al race in 2016 were added.

“It showed that people who were more disgusted by smells were also more likely to vote for Donald Trump than those who were less sensitive,” Olofsson said.

“We thought that was interestin­g because Donald Trump talks frequently about how different people disgust him.

“He thinks that women are disgusting and that immigrants spread disease and it comes up often in his rhetoric.

“It fits with our hypothesis that his supporters would be more easily disgusted themselves.”

The results of the study could be interprete­d to suggest that authoritar­ian political views were innate and difficult to change, but Olofsson believed these could still change, even if deep-seated.

A smile isn’t always welcome

A smile can put us at ease — but not always.

Researcher­s have demonstrat­ed how a quick grin can actually increase physical stress responses in situations where we’re being evaluated — like a job interview — depending on what we perceive that smile to mean.

Psychology studies have already shown how bad remarks from others — like “that wasn’t good” — in everyday scenarios where we’re being tested can activate what’s called the hypothalam­ic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis, otherwise known as our body’s central stress response system.

Until now, little has been known about how we respond to unspoken cues.

US researcher­s have found that seeing smiles that are rewarding or acknowledg­ing can lower our levels of stress hormones.

But seeing “dominating” smiles — which are actually creepy and signal disapprova­l — can push up our stress hormone levels.

Participan­ts in the study who perceived these “dominance” smiles took longer to return to their baseline cortisol levels after the stressful situation was over — a response that was quite similar to getting spoken negative feedback.

The authors, from the University of Wisconsin, also found that individual­s with higher heart-rate variabilit­y — the variation in the time between each heart beat — showed more nuanced responses to different smiles.

They now want to look at whether men and women respond differentl­y to the same kind of smile, and test the physiologi­cal effects of those more overtly negative facial expression­s.

 ?? Pictures / 123RF ??
Pictures / 123RF
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand