Business Day

Sesame Street helped children’s education turn a corner

- TIM HARFORD

The children’s TV show Sesame Street celebrates its 50th birthday this week. My favourite character should be Count von Count, who shares my fondness for numbers. But I’ve always had a soft spot for Mr Snuffleupa­gus, Big Bird’s best friend.

Mr Snuffy was thought by every adult on Sesame Street to be imaginary despite being as real as Elmo. It’s a good joke: Mr Snuffy, a strange anteaterma­mmoth hybrid, is colossal. How could the adults not notice him?

After the gag had run for 14 years, the adults finally realised that Mr Snuffleupa­gus was real, and apologised to Big Bird for doubting him. This was a weighty decision: Sesame

Street’s writers were concerned about child abuse, and reflected it might be unwise to portray the adults as disbelievi­ng what the childlike Big Bird told them.

This was typically painsstaki­ng behaviour from a show that always had ambitious ideas about helping children. In 1967, a former TV producer named Joan Ganz Cooney wrote a report for the Carnegie Corporatio­n titled “The Potential Uses of Television in Preschool Education”. She made the case that carefully crafted television could “foster intellectu­al and cultural developmen­t in pre-schoolers”.

Two years later, her vision became reality in the Children’s Television Workshop, which later became Sesame Street.

It was a radical idea: a few years earlier, Marshall McLuhan had argued that “the medium is the message”. It seemed natural enough to many that TV was an inherently superficia­l medium with, therefore, a superficia­l message. By contrast, Sesame

Street was a bet that good TV could make a real difference to children’s readiness for school, particular­ly for those starved of other opportunit­ies to learn.

Not only would it help them to read and count, but it would be racially integrated. Over the years it would tackle issues including death, divorce, autism, infertilit­y, adoption and HIV.

Researcher­s swarmed all over Sesame Street, trying to figure out whether it actually worked. One early study, conducted by Samuel Ball and Gerry Ann Bogatz, aimed at a convention­al experiment: some families, chosen at random, would be encouraged to sit preschoole­rs in front of the show, while a control group of other families would receive no encouragem­ent.

The problem was that

Sesame Street became so popular, that it was hard to distinguis­h between the two groups; everyone was watching. Neverthele­ss, the study authors found that children who watched more

Sesame Street learnt more, and that “in terms of its own stated goals Sesame Street was in general highly successful”.

Perhaps the message is the message after all. Yet it is hard to be sure about causation. Did

Sesame Street help kids learn? Or was the programme attractive to children who were already flourishin­g?

A study by economists Melissa Kearney and Phillip Levine approaches the problem from a different angle. They noted that in the early years of

Sesame Street some geographic­al areas couldn’t receive the show’s broadcast signals. Two-thirds of US children could watch the show, and many did, but one-third could not. Kearney and Levine concluded that the children who had lived in a region where

Sesame Street was available were less likely to fall behind at school. The effect was about as large as attending the US Head Start early childhood education programme — impressive, given that TV is so cheap. The benefits were particular­ly large for children from deprived areas.

It is hard to read about this study without being reminded that Sesame Street was born in a very different world — one where children received

Sesame Street via UHF broadcast, rather than watching

Baby Shark on YouTube, where a version produced by the South Korean media brand Pinkfong has nearly 4-billion views. Like the Children’s Television

Workshop 50 years ago, Pinkfong has lofty educationa­l goals: its videos are supposed to teach English to Korean children. It has more than twice as many YouTube subscriber­s as Sesame Street, which struggled financiall­y before cutting a deal with HBO.

But the vast, cosmopolit­an and mysterious world of toddler YouTube seems unlikely to deliver the same educationa­l benefits to children as Sesame

Street, which was continuall­y tweaked to help children learn rather than being relentless­ly optimised for the clicks. As Alexis Madrigal observed in a long report on toddler YouTube, the viral videos tend to be fastpaced and full of superfluou­s details. These features may attract the attention of preschoole­rs, but educationa­l experts think they are unhelpful.

I’m an optimist. Online video could surely be even more educationa­l than Sesame Street, given its ability to be interactiv­e and to gather data on a child’s progress. But it would have to be carefully designed and tested, in the same way Sesame Street was. An educationa­l revolution doesn’t happen by accident.

ONLINE VIDEO COULD SURELY BE EVEN MORE EDUCATIONA­L THAN SESAME STREET, GIVEN ITS ABILITY TO BE INTERACTIV­E

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from South Africa