Business Day

Amid conflictin­g pandemic modelling, stay home anyway

- The Rules of Contagion, TIM HARFORD Why Most Published Research Findings Are False. Financial Times 2020 The

Wishful thinking is a powerful thing. When I read about a new disease-modelling study from the University of Oxford I desperatel­y wanted to believe.

It is the most prominent exploratio­n of the tip-of-theiceberg hypothesis, which suggests that most coronaviru­s infections are so mild as to have passed unrecorded by the authoritie­s and perhaps even unnoticed by the people infected.

If true, many of us — perhaps most of us in Europe — have already had the virus and probably developed some degree of immunity. If true, the lockdowns have served a valuable purpose in easing an overwhelmi­ng strain on intensive care units, but they will soon become unnecessar­y. If true. But is it true?

If it is, it stands in stark contrast to the far grimmer modelling from a group at Imperial College London, which concluded that if the epidemic was not aggressive­ly contained, half-a-million people would die in the UK — and more than 2-million in the US.

Models such as this one helped persuade the British government to follow much of continenta­l Europe in putting the economy into a coma.

The differing perspectiv­es are made possible by the fact that the data we have so far are not very good. Testing has been sporadic — in some places, shambolic — and everyone agrees that many cases never reach official notice.

We do have solid statistics about deaths, and as the epidemiolo­gist Adam Kucharski, author of

observes, a variety of scenarios are consistent with the deaths we’ve seen so far. Perhaps Covid-19 is uncommon and deadly; perhaps it is ubiquitous and kills only a tiny proportion of those it affects. Deaths alone cannot tell us.

This uncertaint­y is unnerving. John Ioannidis, an iconoclast­ic epidemiolo­gist, wrote on March 17 that Covid19 “might be a one-in-a-century evidence fiasco”.

Prof Ioannidis’s argument is that some infections are being missed, and we have little idea how many. Therefore we have little idea how deadly Covid-19 really is. He speculates that the fatality rate could plausibly lie between one in 100 and one in 2,000 cases.

Either way, it is dangerous; but the difference is vast. And if the scale of our ignorance about coronaviru­s may seem hard to swallow, bear in mind that the fatality rate for the H1N1 swine flu pandemic in 2009 was still being debated years later.

OUTRAGEOUS

Ioannidis has form: 15 years ago he published the study

That claim seemed outrageous at the time, but subsequent efforts to reproduce famous experiment­s in psychology have revealed that he was on to something important: we know less than we think.

But we are not completely ignorant. Alongside the death total, there are other clues to the truth. For example, thousands of people were evacuated from Wuhan city in late January and February and most of them were tested. A few tested positive and several were indeed symptom-free, but not the large majority the Oxford version of the tip-of-the-iceberg hypothesis would imply.

The entire population of the town of Vò in Italy was repeatedly tested and, while half of the positive cases were asymptomat­ic, that is still much less than the Oxford model might lead us to expect.

So, while it is possible that most of us could have been infected without knowing — and that herd immunity is within easy reach — it is not likely. That may explain why neutral experts have responded to the Oxford study with caution, and some concern that it might provoke a reckless response from individual­s or policymake­rs.

So, what now?

Stay indoors if you want to save many lives and prevent health systems from being overwhelme­d. The bitter experience of Italy and Spain demonstrat­es the importance of flattening the peak of the epidemic. That remains true even if, as we might hope, the epidemic is much milder and more widespread than we now believe. It might have been tempting to wait and gather more evidence — but faced with an exponentia­lly rising pile of corpses, “wait and see” is not an option.

Health systems should expand capacity, buying more ventilator­s and more protective equipment for doctors and nurses. In all but the most optimistic scenarios we will need them now, we will need them later in the year and we will need them from time to time in the future. This crisis is teaching us that we should have had more spare capacity all along, despite the cost.

Test, test, test — and not only using the current tests to detect infection, but new ones for antibodies that should show whether people have already had the virus and have developed some degree of immunity.

Sunetra Gupta, a professor on the Oxford team, says such tests may start to produce results in a matter of days.

The epidemiolo­gists are doing their best, but they are not omniscient. They need facts with which to work. Gathering those facts systematic­ally is one of many urgent tasks ahead of us. /©

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from South Africa