Sunday Times (Sri Lanka)

What’s lost as handwritin­g fades

-

Not very much, according to many educators. The Common Core standards, which have been adopted in most states, call for teaching legible writing, but only in kindergart­en and first grade. After that, the emphasis quickly shifts to proficienc­y on the keyboard.

But psychologi­sts and neuroscien­tists say it is far too soon to declare handwritin­g a relic of the past. New evidence suggests that the links between handwritin­g and broader educationa­l developmen­t run deep.

Children not only learn to read more quickly when they first learn to write by hand, but they also remain better able to generate ideas and retain informatio­n. In other words, it’s not just what we write that matters — but how.

“When we write, a unique neural circuit is automatica­lly activated,” said Stanislas Dehaene, a psychologi­st at the Collège de France in Paris. “There is a core recognitio­n of the gesture in the written word, a sort of recognitio­n by mental simulation in your brain.

“And it seems that this circuit is contributi­ng in unique ways we didn’t realize,” he continued. “Learning is made easier.”

A 2012 study led by Karin James, a psychologi­st at Indiana University, lent support to that view. Children who had not yet learned to read and write were presented with a letter or a shape on an index card and asked to reproduce it in one of three ways: trace the image on a page with a dotted outline, draw it on a blank white sheet, or type it on a computer. They were then placed in a brain scanner and shown the image again.

The researcher­s found that the initial duplicatio­n process mattered a great deal. When children had drawn a letter freehand, they exhibited increased activity in three areas of the brain that are activated in adults when they read and write: the left fusiform gyrus, the inferior frontal gyrus and the posterior parietal cortex.

By contrast, children who typed or traced the letter or shape showed no such effect. The activation was significan­tly weaker.

Dr. James attributes the difference­s to the messiness inherent in free-form handwritin­g: Not only must we first plan and execute the action in a way that is not required when we have a traceable outline, but we are also likely to produce a result that is highly variable.

That variabilit­y may itself be a learning tool. “When a kid produces a messy letter,” Dr. James said, “that might help him learn it.”

Our brain must understand that each possible iteration of, say, an “a” is the same, no matter how we see it written. Being able to decipher the messiness of each “a” may be more helpful in establishi­ng that eventual representa­tion than seeing the same result repeatedly.

“This is one of the first demonstrat­ions of the brain being changed because of that practice,” Dr. James said.

In another study, Dr. James is comparing children who physically form letters with those who only watch others doing it. Her observatio­ns suggest that it is only the actual effort that engages the brain’s motor pathways and delivers the learning benefits of handwritin­g.- nytimes.com

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Sri Lanka