Sunday Times (Sri Lanka)

Why our facial expression­s don't reflect our feelings

For centuries, we’ve believed that facial expression­s mirror our innermost emotions. But recent research has found that may be far from the truth.

- By Talya Rachel Meyers

While conducting research on emotions and facial expression­s in Papua New Guinea in 2015, psychologi­st Carlos Crivelli discovered something startling. He showed Trobriand Islanders photograph­s of the standard Western face of fear – wide- eyed, mouth agape – and asked them to identify what they saw. The Trobriande­rs didn’t see a frightened face. Instead, they saw an indication of threat and aggression. In other words, what we think of as a universal expression of fear isn’t universal at all. But if Trobriande­rs have a different interpreta­tion of facial expression­s, what does that mean?

One emerging – and increasing­ly supported – theory is that facial expression­s don’t reflect our feelings. Instead of reliable readouts of our emotional states, they show our intentions and social goals. The face acts “like a road sign to affect the traffic that’s going past it,” says Alan Fridlund, a psychology professor at University of California Santa Barbara who co-authored a recent study with Crivelli arguing for a more utilitaria­n view of facial expression­s. “Our faces are ways we direct the trajectory of a social interactio­n.”

That’s not to say that we actively try to manipulate others with our facial expression­s ( though every once in a while, we might). Our smiles and frowns may well be instinctiv­e. But our expression­s are less a mirror of what’s going on inside than a signal we’re sending about what we want to happen next. Your best ‘disgusted’ face, for example, might show that you’re not happy with the way the conversati­on is going – and that you want it to take a different tack.

“It’s the only reason that makes sense for facial expression to have evolved,” says Bridget Waller, an evolutiona­ry psychology professor at the University of Portsmouth. Faces, she says, are always “giving some sort of important and useful informatio­n both to the sender… and to the receiver.”

While it may seem sensible, this theory has been a long time coming. The idea that emotions are fundamenta­l, instinctiv­e and expressed in our faces is deeply ingrained in Western culture. Ancient Greeks placed the ‘passions’ in opposition to reason; in the 17th Century, philosophe­r René Descartes laid out six basic passions which could interfere with rational thought.

In the 1960s and ’ 70s, scientific research also began to back up the idea that a few basic emotions could be universall­y understood through facial expression­s.

In different countries around the world, researcher Paul Ekman asked subjects to match photos of facial expression­s with emotions or emotional scenarios. His studies seemed to indicate that some expression­s, and their correspond­ing feelings, were recognised by people of all cultures. ( These “basic emotions” were happiness, surprise, disgust, fear, sadness, and anger.) Today, the legacy of Ekman’s theories is everywhere: from the “Feelings” posters you see in preschools with their cartoons of smiles and frowns to a US government programme designed to identify potential terrorists.

New research is challengin­g two of the main pillars of basic emotion theory. First is the idea that some emotions are universall­y shared and recognised. Second is the belief that facial expression­s are reliable reflectors of those emotions. “They are two different points which have really been confounded by scholars,” says Maria Gendron, a psychology researcher at Northeaste­rn University soon joining the Yale University faculty.

That new research includes recent work by Crivelli. He has spent months immersed with the Trobriande­rs of Papua New Guinea as well as the Mwani of Mozambique. With both indigenous groups, he found that study participan­ts did not attribute emotions to faces in the same way Westerners do.

It was not just the face of fear, either. Shown a smiling face, only a small percentage of Trobriande­rs declared that the face was happy. About half of those who were asked to describe it in their own words called it “laughing”: a word (Courtesy BBC) that deals with action, not feeling. And several described the smiling face as displaying the “magic of attraction”, a uniquely Trobriand-identified emotion that Crivelli describes as “a raptured enchantmen­t”, or a feeling of being positively impacted by magic.

Gendron found similar reactions while studying other indigenous groups – the Himba people in Namibia and the Hadza in Tanzania. Both groups, when asked to describe a facial expression in their own words, tended not to describe an expression as “happy” or “sad”. Instead, they would focus on the actions of the people in the photograph­s or extrapolat­e reasons for the expression­s. In other words, neither researcher found evidence that what is behind a facial expression – including whether an expression reflects an innermost emotion at all – is innately or universall­y understood.

Making matters more complicate­d, even when our facial expression­s are interprete­d by others as exhibiting a certain feeling, they might pinpoint an emotion we’re not actually experienci­ng. In a 2017 analysis of about 50 studies, researcher­s found that only a minority of people’s faces reflected their actual feelings. According to co-author Rainer Reisenzein, there was one strong exception: amusement, which almost always resulted in smiling or laughter. Reisenzein hesitates to interpret what those findings mean. However, he does feel that there are good evolutiona­ry reasons for us not to reveal our inner states to other people: “It puts us at a disadvanta­ge.”

If our expression­s don’t actually reflect our feelings, there are enormous consequenc­es. One is in the field of artificial intelligen­ce (AI), specifical­ly robotics. “A good number of people are training their artificial intelligen­ce and their social robots using these classic ‘poster’ faces,” says Fridlund. But if someone who frowns at a robot is signalling something other than simple unhappines­s, the AI may respond to them incorrectl­y.

“There’s no way to predict how the robot should react when it sees a smiley face or a pouty face or a growling face,” Fridlund points out. “You have to have some kind of knowledge of the person’s role with respect to you, and also your history together, before knowing what that face means.” Fridlund, who consults with companies that develop AI, feels that AI taught to draw from contextual cues will be more effective.

For most of us, though, the new research may have most of an effect on how we interpret social interactio­ns. It turns out that we might communicat­e better if we saw faces not as mirroring hidden emotions – but rather as actively trying to speak to us. People should read faces “kind of like a road sign,” says Fridlund. “It’s like a switch on a railroad track: do we go here or do we go there in the conversati­on?” That scowl on your friend’s face may not be actual anger; maybe she just wants you to agree with her point of view.

Take laughter, says Waller: “when you laugh and how you laugh within a social interactio­n is absolutely crucial.” An inappropri­ately-timed laugh might not reveal your inner joy at what’s going on – but it might show that you’re not paying close attention to the conversati­on.

For Crivelli, our faces may even be more calculatin­g than that. He compares us to puppeteers, with our expression­s like “invisible wires or ropes that you are trying to use to manipulate the other.” And, of course, that other person is manipulati­ng us right back. We’re social creatures, after all. (Courtesy BBC)

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Sri Lanka