Bangkok Post

SPECTACLE OF A LIFETIME

A look back on thrills, agony and drama of Russia World Cup

- DAVID GENDELMAN

>> Goals in football games can be few and far between, which helps explain the delirious nature of most scoring celebratio­ns. Some players yank off their jerseys or drop to their knees and glide across the turf in glee. They all often end up at the bottom of a pile of jubilant teammates.

Then there are the players who are presented with a goal-scoring opportunit­y and, for whatever reason, fail, or who react after a defeat. When this happens, they all do the same thing: raise their hands and place them on their heads.

If you followed the World Cup, you most likely saw it dozens of times, by players from every position and every country.

Argentina’s Lionel Messi did it and so did Portugal’s Cristiano Ronaldo with both missing a penalty against Iceland and Iran respective­ly.

Players of semi-finalists France, Belgium, England and Croatia had the disappoint­ed pose, while some players of defending champions Germany did this after their shock loss to South Korea.

It has nothing to do with football and everything to do with the human psyche, according to zoologists, psychologi­sts and others who study such things.

The gesture signifies that “you know you messed up,” said Jessica Tracy, a professor of psychology at the University of British Columbia. “It’s going to tell others, ‘I get it and I’m sorry, therefore you don’t have to kick me out of the group, you don’t have to kill me’.”

It’s not limited to the shooter, either. In one of the most replayed football blunders of all time, from the 2010 World Cup, Yakubu Aiyegbeni of Nigeria missed an empty net from about 10 feet away. Though Aiyegbeni hardly moved afterwards, nearly every one of his teammates and coaches made the gesture in immediate and unrehearse­d synchronis­ation.

In his seminal 1981 study of the sport, The Soccer Tribe, zoologist Desmond Morris included the gesture in his catalog of 12 player reactions to defeat. He noted its function of self-comfort, which he described as “a form of auto-contact, a widespread device used when the individual feels in need of a reassuring embrace, but has no one immediatel­y available to offer one.” It’s seen among nonhuman primates as well.

In 2008, Tracy published an influentia­l study with her colleague David Matsumoto in which they studied the gestures of both victory and defeat made by sighted and congenital­ly blind Olympic athletes. They found evidence to suggest that the display behaviours of pride and shame were innate and universal.

“You have the head in the hands — that’s shame,” Tracy said. “You have the constricti­on of the body, in the way that the player is moving his arms around his head, almost to make himself smaller. Those are very classical shame display elements.”

No one knows better than the players when they mess up. Cobi Jones, who had a long career with the US men’s national football team and now works as a TV analyst, said in a telephone interview that a blatant miss triggers, along with the gesture, a sense of disbelief and embarrassm­ent. “That’s what we train for, day in and day out, to put that ball in the net,” he said. “And that’s a simple one. It’s one that you shouldn’t miss.”

The gesture is also commonly made after a goalkeeper makes a spectacula­r save to prevent what otherwise would be a sure goal. One of the most famous examples came in the 2006 World Cup final. Late in extra time of a tie game, French star Zinedine Zidane snapped a header that he thought was destined to win the tournament, only to see Italy goalkeeper Gianluigi Buffon nudge the ball over the crossbar with his fingertips. Zidane’s hands went straight to the top of his balding head.

Whether the ball misses the net because of a gaffe by the shooter or a spectacula­r save by the goalkeeper, the response by the rejected players remains nearly identical.

Jones described the offensive player’s experience in both cases as “shock.”

“When people get startled unexpected­ly, their hand will sort of move up to their head almost in a protective motion,” said Dacher Keltner, a professor of psychology at the University of California, Berkeley. “The oldest kind of behavioura­l intention in that class of behaviours is to protect your head from blows.”

The hands-to-the-head gesture is also performed by fans at the same moments as the players. Since they are observers rather than participan­ts, their motivation­s could differ. Philip Furley, a lecturer in sports psychology at the German Sports University, in Cologne, has studied players’ behaviour during penalty kicks, when the gesture is common.

Among the spectators, Furley said, “what you often find is this sort of contagion going on. If it’s a team you support, then if this player you’re supporting in this moment shows something you might be infected by his nonverbal behaviour.”

No matter the cause, the near absolute predictabi­lity of the gesture has become its most defining trait. “It’s like punch lines, catchphras­es that comedians use,” Goldblatt said. “People start laughing before you say them. A lot of comics work on that.”

 ??  ??
 ??  ?? ABOVE Germany players react after losing to South Korea at the World Cup.
ABOVE Germany players react after losing to South Korea at the World Cup.
 ??  ?? RIGHT Portugal’s Cristiano Ronaldo, centre, reacts after missing a penalty against Iran during a group match at Russia 2018.
RIGHT Portugal’s Cristiano Ronaldo, centre, reacts after missing a penalty against Iran during a group match at Russia 2018.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Thailand