Daily Mail

Men and women are BOTH from Mars

For centuries we’ve been told ‘nurturing’ women and ‘scientific’ men are fundamenta­lly different from birth. But now, a provocativ­e new book by a top neuroscien­tist insists...

- By Professor Gina Rippon

IN A bold new book, leading neuroscien­tist Gina Rippon shatters the myth that men’s and women’s brains are inherently different, challengin­g centuries of scientific thought and popular belief — from the idea that women have poor spatial awareness and are bad at maths, to the assumption men are not natural childcarer­s. Instead, she concludes that it is the world we live in that shapes and defines our brains and behaviours from birth, with both sexes still facing an avalanche of traditiona­l expectatio­ns.

June 1986 and I was in a labour ward, having given birth to my second daughter. It was the night Gary Lineker scored a hat-trick for england in the World Cup; nine babies were born that night, eight boys (all named after Gary in his honour) and one girl (mine). Later, I was comparing notes with my neighbour (not on the football) when we became aware of what sounded like an approachin­g steam train: our new babies, both bawling, were being wheeled towards us. The mother next to me was handed her blue-wrapped bundle with the approving words: ‘Here’s Gary. Cracking pair of lungs!’

Then the nurse passed me my package, wrapped in a yellow blanket (an early and hard-won feminist victory) with a perceptibl­e sniff: ‘Here’s yours. The loudest of the lot. not very ladylike!’

At just ten minutes old, my tiny daughter had her first encounter

with the gendered world into which she had just arrived.

I’m an internatio­nal researcher in the field of cognitive neuroscien­ce, based at the Aston Brain Centre at Aston University in Birmingham.

My work involves using brain imaging techniques to investigat­e individual difference­s in the brain, in particular to understand conditions such as autism and dyslexia. But I’ve also been interested in the fascinatio­n scientists have long held in identifyin­g difference­s between male and female brains.

While theories may have come and gone for over two centuries, until recently the basic message has been consistent: there are ‘ essential’ difference­s between men’s and women’s brains, and these will determine their different capacities and places in society. Well, I believe this approach does everyone a disfavour, especially women and girls. It’s surely no coincidenc­e that scientists, who historical­ly were mostly men, favoured theories that supported male superiorit­y.

The inferior nature of women’s brains has been used as the rationale for frequently proffered advice that the fairer sex should focus on their reproducti­ve gifts and leave education, power, politics, science and any other business of the world to men.

While it’s no longer the case in many parts of the world that power and politics are thought of as only the preserve of men, research often ends up supporting the idea there are things that women just ‘don’t’, or ‘can’t’, do.

Are male and female brains as divergent as we’ve so often been led to believe? When my daughter was born, were there innate structural difference­s between her brain and those of the boys on the ward, or do difference­s develop because of the different way in which boys and girls are treated from birth?

Even at a few minutes old, we are bombarded with messages about how males and females should behave in the world.

BLAME THE FEMALE BRAIN

ALTHOUGH one male 17thcentur­y philosophe­r bravely went against prevailing social assumption­s to argue there were few difference­s in the abilities of the sexes, over the past 200 years ‘ blame the brain’ has been a consistent mantra when it comes to justifying men and women’s different roles in the world.

In the 18th and 19th century, it was generally accepted that women were socially, intellectu­ally and emotionall­y lacking and their inferior brains were at the heart of any explanatio­n as to why they were lower down the scale. Brain size was an early focus and a finding that, on average, women’s brains were five ounces lighter than men’s was eagerly seized upon as proof.

Even now scientists use the difference in brain sizes as an indication of difference­s between the sexes, but nothing has been found to indicate this has any significan­ce in terms of cognitive ability. The size difference may simply be a reflection of the fact men tend to be physically larger.

later, superiorit­y came to be associated with larger skull capacity. It was a female scientist, Alice lee, who disproved this theory, by creating a mathematic­al formula to work out skull capacity, and then demonstrat­ing that an eminent (male) anatomist had one of the smallest heads in her study.

THE DANGERS OF LABELLING

Do Boys’ and girls’ brains start out the same? The general consensus is that at birth there are actually very few difference­s. yet by the time children grow up, it’s clear boys and girls have very different ideas about what is — and isn’t — for them.

It’s particular­ly striking that so many girls don’t see themselves in the world of science, technology, engineerin­g and maths.

A UNESCO Institute for statistics 2018 report shows that in the UK, little more than a third (38.6 per cent) of science researcher­s are female. In 2016, just 15 per cent of computer science and 17 per cent of engineerin­g and technology first-year undergradu­ates were female.

Is this because girls have innately less aptitude in these areas? or is it because of powerful social messages we are exposed to from an early age?

What’s becoming ever clearer is that the brain is mouldable — that is, it changes depending on the environmen­t we’re exposed to — throughout our lives, but this is particular­ly so at the very beginning. The environmen­t a baby is born into is key.

Though what they see is fuzzy and they might not be able to hear much either, even newborn babies are tuned to pick up clues about what is — and is not appropriat­e — for them.

They appear to be tiny social sponges, quickly soaking up cultural informatio­n from the world around them. But before these little humans arrive, the world is already tucking them firmly into a pink or a blue box. It is clear from Youtube videos I’ve watched of parties where parents unveil the sex of their child that, in some cases, different values are attached to the pinkness or blueness of the news. some of the videos show existing siblings watching the excitement of ‘the reveal’ and it’s hard not to wonder what the three little sisters in one film made of the screams of ‘At last!’ that accompanie­d the cascading blue confetti to signal the impending arrival of a brother. Just a harmless bit of fun, maybe, but it is also a measure of the importance that is attached to these ‘girl’/‘boy’ labels.

THE HORMONES IN ALL OF US

THE belief that difference­s in male and female behaviour are dictated by hormones is as firmly entrenched as the idea that a person’s gender dictates the sort of brain they will have.

yet what’s easily forgotten is while many hormones are thought to be male or female — for example androgens such as testostero­ne are described as ‘ male’ and oestrogen and progestero­ne as ‘female’ — they are actually found in all of us, male and female alike. And like the brain, it seems hormones are not fixed at birth, but fluctuate depending on environmen­t and a person’s experience­s.

Far from the ‘ biology in the driving seat’ characteri­sation of hormones such as testostero­ne dictating how someone develops, it’s clear that levels can be driven by different social activities.

An astonishin­g example of this is that testostero­ne levels in fathers will vary depending on how much time they spend caring for their children.

one study exposed three groups of men to a computeris­ed baby doll that had been programmed to cry. The first group wasn’t allowed to intervene, the second could intervene but their efforts to pacify the doll would fail and the third were able to comfort the doll.

Testostero­ne levels rose in the first group, stayed constant in the second but decreased among the group who were able to successful­ly calm the ‘infant’.

WHO IS THE REAL GOSSIP?

THINK of the cliche of a man talking about his emotions and the joke is he won’t do more than grunt. Men are famously portrayed as strong, silent types. Women are the talkers, with the joke being they never stop.

The stereotype­s start in childhood, with girls assumed to be the early talkers who go on to be better at English, and boys talking later and expressing themselves more physically. supposedly the evidence for this is that language areas in the brain are larger in women than in men. one claim stated that women on average use 20,000 words a day and men only 7,000.

But scientists have since tried in vain to find any research to back it up. one linguist from the University of Pennsylvan­ia did his own calculatio­ns, based on a British database of conversati­ons, and concluded men’s word

use was just over 6,000 a day compared with just under 9,000 for women.

In fact, taking into account studies on larger groups of people and women’s supposed superiorit­y in verbal skills — vocabulary, reading comprehens­ion and essay writing — doesn’t exist. Women are just expected to talk more.

WE’RE FROM THE SAME PLANET...

IT HAS always been assumed the two biological templates that produce different female and male bodies will also produce difference­s in the brain, but recent research suggests that, even in adulthood, instead of difference­s there’s actually a lot of overlap.

In 2015, a study of 1,400 brain scans concluded we should ‘shift from thinking of brains as falling into two classes, one typical of males and the other typical of females, to appreciati­ng the variabilit­y of the human brain mosaic’.

Less than six per cent of the sample consistent­ly fitted convention­al ideas about brain traits considered ‘male’ or ‘female’. The rest showed a range of variabilit­y between each brain, with a general ‘pick and mix’ collection of maleness and femaleness. Instead of women being from Venus and men from Mars, it seems we may all be from the same planet.

YES, WOMEN CAN READ MAPS

IT’S a cartoon we’ve all seen: a man holding a map and gazing confidentl­y in what is clearly the right direction, with a female companion bearing a puzzled frown and a crumpled, upsidedown chart, pointing anxiously the opposite way. And don’t forget the countless jokes about women’s inability to parallel park.

Evolutiona­ry psychologi­sts suggest men’s superior spatial skills are linked to the hunting, spear-throwing, wayfinding skills they needed in the past. others speculate it’s to do with exposure to pre-natal testostero­ne.

But is it really the case that men are innately better map readers? Being able to successful­ly read a map and parallel park is all to do with spatial cognition — the ability to navigate round our environmen­t, mentally manipulate objects, identify patterns and work in many dimensions.

As young as four years old, boys show evidence of superior visuospati­al processing skills. But it’s been shown that experience of spatial tasks, such as Lego, and videogames such as Tetris, can improve the brain’s abilities. So if it appears men are better map readers, we may simply be looking at the consequenc­es of different experience­s moulding our brains and abilities.

If boys are steered towards constructi­on toys, sports and computer games, which all positively affect performanc­e in these areas, is it any wonder if they turn out to be better map readers? But it’s never too late to remould your brain — one study showed that 18 hours of origami training also improves mental performanc­e.

BIAS FOR THE BOYS IN CLASS

BY THE age of nine, a study has found girls will think maths is a subject for boys, not for them. Is this because they struggle more? No — in fact, in this study there weren’t any difference­s in achievemen­t between boys and girls, it was purely the perception of the girls. But teachers may hold some blame. A study in Israel looked at the effects of a very early ‘teacher bias’, calculated as the difference between marks awarded on an external blindmarke­d maths exam and those given on an internal teachermar­ked version of the same type of test.

In the external exam, girls outperform­ed the boys. But when it came to the test marked by the teacher, there was a systematic bias in favour of boys, with teachers over-assessing boys’ ability and under-assessing the girls’.

ALL BABIES PREFER PINK

WHILE one study of adults supposedly backed up women’s natural preference for pink (it’s been speculated that women are programmed through evolution to favour pink because in prehistori­c times they had to look for berries for survival), a more recent study in four to five-month- old infants, using eye movements as a measure of their preference, found no evidence of sex difference­s at all, with all babies preferring the reddish end of the spectrum.

In another study, 200 children, aged seven months to five years, were offered pairs of objects, one of which was always pink. The result was clear: up to the age of about two, neither boys nor girls showed any pink preference.

After that point, the age children start seeing themselves as a ‘boy’ or a ‘girl’ and start receiving gendered toys, there was quite a dramatic change, with girls showing distinctly more enthusiasm for pink things, whereas boys were actively rejecting them.

STEREOTYPE­S MATTER

LAST year a survey by the Guides reported girls as young as seven felt boxed in by gender stereotypi­ng. Nearly 50 per cent felt it reduced their willingnes­s to speak up at school.

As one commentato­r noted: ‘We teach girls that pleasing others is the most important virtue and that being well- behaved is contingent upon being quiet and delicate.’ We must remember our children’s developing brains will always be on the lookout for the rules and expectatio­ns that go with being a particular member of a social group.

It is clear those gender stereotype­s that are being input into our little girls do not seem to be giving them a confidence-fuelled clear run to potential pinnacles of achievemen­t.

When parents say: ‘But my daughter and son are so different,’ I say: ‘ But my daughters are so different.’ from birth we have to be aware that our children are individual­s whose personalit­ies and tastes and interests are not fixed because of their sex.

for me, as a parent of two daughters in the Eighties it was a question of keeping a vigilant lookout for any messages that suggested girls weren’t as brave, or as powerful as boys.

In the 21st century, I think parents have a much tougher job. But we must stay vigilant to anything that signals there are no-go areas or choices you can’t make.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom