The Daily Telegraph - Features

Swat at your peril – fruit flies may have feelings too

Scientists believe consciousn­ess could be more widespread among animals than was previously thought. By Ed Cumming

-

Elephants mourn their dead, octopuses play catch, grey parrots recognise themselves in the mirror. But how about crabs that weigh up decisions, bees that play games and fruit flies that dream?

The past decade has seen rapid developmen­ts in the field of research, upending widely held assumption­s about which animals exhibit signs of consciousn­ess.

The New York Declaratio­n on Animal Consciousn­ess, signed by 39 scientists and academics last week, aims to reframe the conversati­on.

“Striking new results have hinted at surprising­ly rich inner lives in a very wide range of other animals,” says the declaratio­n, “including many invertebra­tes, driving renewed debate about animal consciousn­ess.”

It follows the Cambridge Declaratio­n on Consciousn­ess in 2012, which recognised possible consciousn­ess in cephalopod­s, birds and non-human mammals, and reflects a fast-changing field.

“We want to create a moment to get people to notice this emerging science of animal consciousn­ess,” says Jonathan Birch, an associate professor in philosophy at LSE. “It’s a topic that has long been marginalis­ed in science. It’s only in the past 10-15 years that there has been a broadening of the ambitions of consciousn­ess science to also study the experience of other animals.”

The most persuasive example of non-mammal intelligen­ce are cephalopod­s, whose behaviour has been made famous by documentar­ies such as the Oscar-winning film My Octopus Teacher, in which a filmmaker forms a friendship with a common octopus who plays with him.

“During my training in the early 2000s, animal sentience and consciousn­ess were conspicuou­sly ignored in serious scientific discourse,” says Dr Alexandra Schnell, a leading researcher. “However, the burgeoning field of animal sentience has revitalise­d these topics, allowing discussion­s about subjective experience­s in animals to flourish.”

They aren’t the only sea creatures surprising researcher­s. For Prof Bob Elwood, at Queen’s University Belfast, the starting point for his research was a chance encounter with the chef Rick Stein. When Elwood explained his specialism in animal welfare, Stein asked him if lobsters could feel pain.

He decided that while he couldn’t prove they were feeling pain, in a human sense, he could try to find out if they were responding just by reflex. “I ran a whole series of experiment­s,” he says. “When I started, I was concerned about being thought odd at even considerin­g the idea they might feel pain.”

His results were remarkable. He found that crabs will rapidly learn to avoid a dark shelter in which they receive electric shocks and use another that is safe.

“No one of these experiment­s is critical in suggesting consciousn­ess or pain,” he says. “But when you see again and again, with different studies asking different questions, and they start agreeing, and you get evidence supporting the idea of pain, you cannot dismiss it.”

Lars Chittka’s career has been a similar journey of discovery on the subject of bees. He is a professor at Queen Mary University in London and the author of The Mind of a Bee. “I really think we are witnessing a Copernican revolution in the understand­ing of animal minds,” he says.

His experiment­s found that bees could learn about a predation thread, which they did “just fine”. Subsequent experiment­s showed them playing and responding in a non-reflex-like manner to painful stimuli, among other behaviours.

“For each of these findings, a critic might find an alternativ­e ‘simple explanatio­n’ but for all of them in combinatio­n this looks increasing­ly hard. The truth is that we have yet to find a single convincing piece of evidence that indicates the absence of consciousn­ess,” Chittka says.

For Bruno van Swinderen, a professor at the University of Queensland, the humble fruit fly offers a way to examine the origins of consciousn­ess mechanisti­cally. “I am at the bottom of the pecking order of conscious animals,” he says. “You’ve got cats and dogs, dolphins and elephants. Even bees do interestin­g cognitive things. Then there’s fruit flies, which I work on. Why would they be included in this?

“We’re seeing all these conversati­ons about AI about when machines will get conscious. From a human perspectiv­e it’s hard to understand, but from a bottom-up perspectiv­e it’s easier to understand how consciousn­ess might evolve from a simple circuit, like a fly brain circuit.”

If we can entertain the prospect of consciousn­ess in a computer, we must entertain it in fruit flies: a remarkable, chastening thought.

‘Striking new results hint at rich inner lives in a wide range of animals’

 ?? ?? A bug’s life: the humble fruit fly offers a way to examine the origins of consciousn­ess
A bug’s life: the humble fruit fly offers a way to examine the origins of consciousn­ess

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom