BBC Science Focus

PROF REBECCA SAXE

A study using fMRI scanners has found that just 10 hours of total isolation can leave the brain feeling starved

- Prof Rebecca Saxe neuroscien­tist SURELY STUDYING SOMEONE REQUIRES SOME DEGREE OF SOCIAL INTERACTIO­N?

A study has shown that loneliness can trigger a craving response in our brains that is similar to hunger. Rebecca Saxe, a professor of cognitive neuroscien­ce at MIT, gives us the lowdown.

HOW DO YOU STUDY CRAVINGS, WHEN THEY ARE SO SUBJECTIVE?

A standard way is to study hunger. You have people fast for much longer than they typically would if they were hungry, then you show them pictures of the food they would like to eat, and you get a craving response in the brain.

Subjective­ly, people can tell you how they feel. But there is also a small, fine strWctWre deep in the Drain called

the ‘substantia nigra’ which produces dopamine. It seems to be the best measure we have as a source of that feeling of craving. What we wanted to do in this study was to see whether that same neural circuit would be involved if you’d been isolated and given a reminder of what you were missing.

HOW DID YOU GO ABOUT IT?

Each volunteer agreed to do one day where they didn’t eat all day and one day where they didn’t see people or interact with people in any way – neither virtually nor live.

At the end of each day, we then showed them pictures of their favourite foods, and of their favourite social activities. [Using an MRI scanner], we measured brain activity in the substantia nigra to see if we could reproduce what we already expected – that there would be a bigger response to food when the volunteers were hungry, and whether that same brain region would show a response to pictures of social interactio­n when they had spent a day being isolated.

WHAT DID YOU FIND?

In the substantia nigra, the main area we were looking at, we found that both food cues when you’re hungry and social cues when you’re isolated evoke this basic craving response. This is incredibly interestin­g, that it worked in a straightfo­rward, similar way [whether hungry for food or hungry for social interactio­n].

WERE YOU SURPRISED THAT THE EFFECT WAS SEEN AFTER JUST ONE DAY?

When we designed the study, one of the main things that we debated was whether 10 hours would be long enough. And also, would the volunteers still feel isolated if they knew exactly when it was going to end? We had to tell our participan­ts what they were volunteeri­ng for, so they knew it was only a day.

We found that, for most of us, a day of isolation does produce cravings in the brain. But it affects different people to different degrees.

A whole puzzle was how do you get a person into an MRI machine without social interactio­n? Normally, participat­ing in scientific research is

a very social experience. We will talk to you about what we’re doing, why

“We found that both food cues when you’re hungry and social cues when you’re isolated evoke this craving response”

we’re doing it and what you should expect. We’ll then talk you through the process of getting into the MRI machine and will make sure that you’re okay #ctWally figWring oWt hoY to get

a person into an MRI scanner, without any social interactio­n, was the main challenge of the studies. We had to train the people in advance to get themselves into the scanner.

We had the minimum number of researcher­s involved and had them gowned and masked so that the volunteers couldn’t see them or see any social interactio­n between them.

CAN YOUR EXPERIMENT TELL US ANYTHING ABOUT THE CURRENT SITUATION IN WHICH MANY PEOPLE ARE SELF-ISOLATING?

When we started out, we just couldn’t imagine that there would be a context in which people would become isolated in this way. But now here we are in this semi-isolated state that will go on for weeks and weeks. However, the isolation that our participan­ts experience­d was much more acute. What we’re experienci­ng [with COVID-19] is a longer term, more mixed isolation where there are some people around and we have access to all forms of media, including our phones. But for the 10 hours of isolation that our participan­ts experience­d, they didn’t have any interactio­n from anybody at all.

ARE THERE ANY IMMEDIATE IMPLICATIO­NS TO THE FINDINGS?

We were inspired by a study that had done a similar thing in mice. And what we found in our study is that regions involved in craving social interactio­n is analogous to what our collaborat­ors have found when they studied it in mice. That result was a key part of our original motivation, because there are research labs all over the world that use mice as the main experiment­al animal to test new ideas about mental health treatments, especially drug treatments. For example, studying social motivation in mice is a way people can test potential treatments for autism.

DO YOU HAVE PLANS FOR FUTURE STUDIES?

The post-doc in my lab is moving to Cambridge University this summer to follow up on the research in a bunch of ways, including looking at how

of isolation. She will also look at the experience­s of adolescent­s, because despite being wildly connected on social media, adolescent­s actually report really high levels of loneliness.

One of the things we said in the paper, which is really important, is that although we can objectivel­y isolate people, subjective loneliness is very different. Being alone doesn’t always mean being lonely, and being social media inflWences experience­s

your social needs. She is particular­ly interested in the causes and predictors of loneliness in adolescent­s and that’s what she’s going to study next year.

PROF REBECCA SAXE

Rebecca is professor of cognitive neuroscien­ce and associate department head at the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT. Interviewe­d by BBC Science Focus commission­ing editor Jason Goodyer. connected is not alYays enoWgh to fWlfil

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ?? COVID-19 has forced many of us into isolation, but we can still enjoy interactio­n with loved ones through our phones
COVID-19 has forced many of us into isolation, but we can still enjoy interactio­n with loved ones through our phones

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom