The Herald on Sunday

Have we finally taken Leave of our senses?

- By Stephen Naysmith Social Affairs Correspond­ent

Bitter arguments, family splits, opposing views shouted down, politician­s harangued in the street, facts disregarde­d in favour of entrenched dogma – it’s like the country’s not thinking straight over Brexit. But why is this happening? We’ve looked into the past to find out ...

“PLEASE don’t’ listen to the Brexiteers’ madness,” Airbus chief executive Tom Enders said on Thursday, while warning there was no guarantee the company would retain its base in Britain after March 29.

That the Brexit debate has been irrational is also a common theme in the Foreign press. “Isle of Madness”, trumpeted Der Spiegel in November. A few weeks earlier, Canada’s Globe and Mail had declared: “A self-destructiv­e madness grips the UK.”

And it is not just Brexiteers who stand accused. Remainers and campaigner­s for a People’s Vote or a second referendum have been branded deluded and dangerous enemies of the people.

But if we have all taken leave of our senses, there may be a psychologi­cal explanatio­n. And while research carried out in Scotland and published this week gives fresh insight into this collective act of self- harm, the original theories date back to the work of a Scottish journalist, poet and songwriter born 200 years ago and raised in an orphanage for Scots children in London.

Charles Mackay is best remembered for his 1841 book Extraordin­ary Popular Delusions And The Madness of Crowds, which is still cited today as one of the earliest studies of how large numbers of people can participat­e in very bad decisions.

So what is the Madness of Crowds?

MACKAY’S three-volume work highlighte­d examples of mass hysteria, from economic booms and subsequent busts such as the South Sea Bubble to witch hunts and the popular expectatio­n that alchemists might one day turn base metals into gold. It looked to explain why large population­s or communitie­s sometimes embrace irrational beliefs and ideas, often in defiance of a blindingly obvious reality.

“We find that whole communitie­s suddenly fix their minds upon one object and go mad in its pursuit,” Mackay wrote. “Millions of people become simultaneo­usly impressed with one delusion and run after it, till their attention is caught by some new folly more captivatin­g than the first.”

The theory has been much studied, including in a more recent work part inspired by Mackay, US journalist James Surowiecki’s 2004 book The Wisdom Of Crowds, Why The Many Are Smarter Than The Few. Surowiecki argued that through sharing informatio­n, groups can make better collective decisions than any individual within them could.

But they can’t both be right?

THE problem is there are good examples to support both theories, but we don’t know how or why “collective wisdom” sometimes works and sometimes goes badly wrong.

That is why new research carried out at St Andrews University is so timely. It explains not only how collective madness can grip a population, leading to bad decisions, but also why the more complex the task, the more likely humans will act like animals, resorting to mimicry and “groupthink”.

According to lead author Wataru Toyokawa, postdoctor­al research fellow at St Andrews University, the natural world can help explain a puzzle which has long troubled social scientists. “Many people have studied how individual­s become more stupid when they behave collective­ly,” he says. “Seeking the opinion of others is normally beneficial, if informatio­n gathering is good. We rely on that for the democratic process,” he says. “But it is a doubleedge­d sword. Copying can be healthy but if all the people are copying each other the system collapses.”

Along with Professor Kevin Laland of St Andrews’ School of Biology and PhD student Andrew Whalen, Toyokawa used an online experiment and population dynamics to explore the point at which the benefits of sharing informatio­n and collaborat­ing to find solutions can tip over into a phenomenon known to psychologi­sts as “maladaptiv­e herding”.

Participan­ts were given a variety of tasks and researcher­s discovered that the larger and more complex the task, the more likely people were to copy those around them, leading to poor

We saw a spike in hate crime after the referendum, that’s never really receded. So there’s always a possibilit­y people are being radicalise­d by the kind of febrile atmosphere we’ve got at the moment

decisions. With Brexit the task is very difficult because each individual has different preference­s, making it hard to reach consensus. In such a political situation, the danger is bad ideas can flourish, Toyokawa says.

What’s happening to decision-making?

OVER the years, research into Mackay’s ideas has consolidat­ed around an understand­ing that conformity has a big influence on decision making. So if enough people believe leaving the EU will lead to a £350 million dividend for the NHS, or reduce overall migration rather than just the numbers coming from EU countries, others are likely to embrace the same ideas more readily, even if they are irrational. This applies especially if the suspect ideas chime with their own views or prejudices.

Science writer Philip Ball is the author of Critical Mass, which considers whether physics and chemistry can tell us as much about crowd behaviour as psychology and social science. This applies to physical crowds and traffic which can move about in ways which are similar to those of atoms in a confined space, he suggests, but can also apply to opinion formation.

“There is a large body of work highlighti­ng the way that crowds can collective­ly make a good decision but can also reach a horrendous­ly wrong one, “he says.

A good example is a “guess the weight of the cake” competitio­n at a country fair. If people know what others have guessed there will often be a conformity effect, with people clustering their guesses close to one of those made earlier. But that earlier guess may be entirely wrong, distorting the outcome. Meanwhile, studies have found that if people guess “blind” in such a contest, averaging out their guesses usually provides a remarkably accurate estimate of the true answer. That is the wisdom of crowds.

“What this tells us is that once you start introducin­g interactio­n between people you are much more likely to find ‘herding’ behaviour, reducing the chance of a ‘good’ guess,” Ball says.

Social media and online manipulati­on

THE rise of the internet and social media has exacerbate­d the dangers, and polarised debate.

This was perhaps seen most markedly in October 2016 when the Supreme Court ruled against the UK Government, saying Parliament should have a say over the triggering of Article 50. Newspapers suggested this was a clash between the courts and the populace, with the Daily Mail picturing the three judges who made the ruling, lined up like criminals’ mugshots, under the headline “enemies of the people”.

This was the birth of the now widelydisc­ussed view that any deviation from Brexit will be a betrayal of voters in the referendum. It was ridiculed by others as an extraordin­ary over-reaction to an entirely legitimate ruling from the English courts – which is where Brexiteers apparently want legal decisions to be made in future.

Ball says: “The questions now are about how these opinion dynamics play out when an overall bias in society can tilt things so markedly in one direction or another. Advertisin­g does that but with social media you see it even more strongly. The echo chamber effect is huge, with people getting feedback from like-minded people. People repost ideas that tally with their own perception­s and fake news can spread like wildfire. Little things can set in train an avalanche effect.”

“This is why we have representa­tive democracy rather than plebiscite­s. If you don’t have a robust system referenda are vulnerable because it only takes a small nudge to send people herding in a certain way.”

But it raises questions about how long democracy can continue to function, he says, especially in light of the fact that many politician­s have been slow to realise the game is changing. “It isn’t just huge billboards, it is the fact that a Cambridge Analytica can understand and predict the effects of targeting certain people, but they also know how to predict and manipulate them.

Are we ants? Or bees?

IN the insect world, the models for the wisdom of the crowd are creatures like ants and bees, both generally regarded as shining examples of collective decision-making. But bees have the edge, Toyokawa explains, and ants are more vulnerable to moments of “madness”.

“Ants normally do very well. They copy and make very accurate decisions – for example, when finding good food and a site for a nest. But sometimes a stupid ant can find a mediocre place. He or she may start advertisin­g this option as good, and other ants react to that and follow.

“So the ants do really well when an environmen­t is stable, but find it different to switch to a new environmen­t. The honey bee generates collective intelligen­ce very well, even in a changing environmen­t.

“When the task is easy, humans behave more like a honey bee. When the group size increases and the task is difficult, we behave more like ants.”

A drift towards extremism

THIS week Neil Basu, assistant commission­er of the Metropolit­an Police, warned Brexit could lead to another surge in far-right extremist groups, views and actions. The Brexit vote itself was followed by a surge in racist attacks, he pointed out, claiming that leaving the EU with no deal would be very bad for policing.

“We saw a spike in hate crime after the referendum, that’s never really receded. So there’s always a possibilit­y people are being radicalise­d by the kind of febrile atmosphere we’ve got at the moment,” he explained.

Toyokawa says group dynamics can explain how people on both sides are draw into face-offs in the street, or the heckling and harassment of politician­s and journalist­s, when they would not otherwise be.

“Shouting in the street may affect other people’s decision-making – we don’t know the answer to that. But the idea of maladaptiv­e herding could certainly apply to demonstrat­ions,” the researcher says. “People who might not usually get involved can end up copying the behaviour of others, just because they’ve seen there is a demo going on.”

This may be more a case of people being emboldened in their original views and prejudices, Ball says, as opposed to flocking mistakenly to bad ideas. “There are ideas in some quarters such as the far right which are being played on and manipulate­d.

“Just as Donald Trump has to a degree legitimise­d racism in America, if people see their views endorsed they can be emboldened.

“If people at the top say it is OK to send out vans with messages on the side, there is the opportunit­y for people on the far right to manipulate the same sentiments. I’m not sure you need a lot of psychology to explain that.”

And he warns that we can no longer ignore the increasing prevalence of such collective irrational­ity.

“We need to recognise these kind of dynamics in order to figure out what a democracy means and how it can operate in this networked age. Does it still have the same meaning?”

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ?? If two heads are better than one, then millions should never make any mistakes. Not so. Just ask the ants ...
If two heads are better than one, then millions should never make any mistakes. Not so. Just ask the ants ...

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom