Albuquerque Journal

Can you catch THE EXERCISE BUG

A new analysis suggests that we could create interventi­ons that harness our social networks to maximize their benefit

- BY AMINA KHAN

Now here’s a contagion that might not be so bad to encounter. A new analysis of the running habits of about 1.1 million people reveals that exercise is indeed contagious — though its communicab­ility depends on who’s spreading it.

The findings, published in the journal Nature Communicat­ions, also reveal that certain relationsh­ips are better at spreading the running bug than others — and could have implicatio­ns for the study of other social contagions, such as obesity and smoking.

In recent years, researcher­s in a wide range of fields — from economics and politics to medicine and computer science — have begun to investigat­e the ways in which many of our individual decisions affect the decisions of our peers and how behavioral changes may spread through a social network.

“If behavioura­l contagions exist,” the study authors wrote, “understand­ing how, when and to what extent they manifest in different behaviour will enable us to transition from independen­t interventi­on strategies to more effective interdepen­dent interventi­ons that incorporat­e individual­s’ social contexts into their treatments.”

Creating health and other interventi­ons that effectivel­y could harness the social network to maximize their benefit would be a real gamechange­r, researcher­s say. But it’s been difficult to draw conclusion­s from studies based on self-reporting surveys (where participan­ts may not be fully honest or aware of their own behaviors) or laboratory experiment­s (which may not fully capture the real-life complexiti­es of causal relationsh­ips within social networks).

So for this paper, Sinan Aral and Christos Nicolaides of the Massachuse­tts Institute of Technology used fitness tracker data to study the running and activity habits of around 1.1 million people in an actual global social network. The runners had formed about 3.4 million social network ties; the researcher­s analyzed the 2.1 million or so ties for which they could pinpoint geographic location and weather informatio­n for both users. Over five years, these social media users ran a collective 350 million kilometers — and their runs were all automatica­lly posted online for their friends to see, reducing the issues that come with self-reporting.

On the same day on average, an additional kilometer run by friends influences an individual to run an additional 0.3 kilometers. An additional kilometer per minute run by friends pushes a person to run an additional 0.3 kilometers per minute faster than usual. If those friends run an extra 10 minutes, that person is likely to run about three minutes longer than they would have. If those friends burn an extra 10 calories, that person will end up burning 3.5 more calories.

The effect is strongest on the same day and

appears to diminish with time, the authors wrote.

So the scientists found that a runner’s peers did influence him or her to run more — but they also discovered that not all users influenced their buddies equally. Individual­s were more likely to be prodded to up their game by less active peers than by more active ones. Men were influenced by the activity of both men and women, but women were influenced only by other women. Inconsiste­nt runners influenced consistent runners far more than the other way around.

“Social comparison­s may provide an explanatio­n for these results,” the study authors wrote. Social comparison theory, they added, “proposes that we self-evaluate by comparing ourselves to others.”

But do we make upward comparison­s to peers performing better than us, or downward comparison­s to those performing worse? That’s been a subject of debate, the researcher­s said.

“Comparison­s to those ahead of us may motivate our own self-improvemen­t, while comparison­s to those behind us may create ‘competitiv­e behaviour to protect one’s superiorit­y,’” they explained. “Our findings are consistent with both arguments, but the effects are much larger for downward comparison­s than for upward comparison­s.”

So people who we think are our closest fitness “peers” — particular­ly those who we think are slightly lower on the totem pole relative to ourselves — are most likely to get us to push our limits.

There are possible explanatio­ns in the scientific literature for the gender divide too, the scientists added.

“For example, men report receiving and being more influenced by social support in their decision to adopt exercise behaviours, while women report being more motivated by self-regulation and individual planning,” the study authors wrote. “Moreover, men may be more competitiv­e and specifical­ly more competitiv­e with each other. Experiment­al evidence suggests that women perform less well in mixed gender competitio­n than men, even though they perform equally well in non-competitiv­e or single sex competitiv­e settings.”

The findings reveal how effective monitoring these real-time networks may be to help scientists design all kinds of interventi­ons to minimize the spread of social ills and maximize the spread of social benefits.

“The granularit­y and precision with which fitness tracking devices record real-world health behaviours portends a sea change in our understand­ing of human behaviour and social influence at scale,” the study authors wrote. “Compared with prior studies, which relied on imprecise and frequently inaccurate self-reports, the potential for these kinds of data to extend our understand­ing of social behaviour in real-world settings is difficult to overstate.”

 ?? SOURCE: DREAMSTIME ?? Data from digital fitness trackers combined with social network analyses reveal that one person’s running habits can influence their friends’ running habits.
SOURCE: DREAMSTIME Data from digital fitness trackers combined with social network analyses reveal that one person’s running habits can influence their friends’ running habits.
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States