China Daily Global Weekly

Getting the facts right on honesty

Researcher­s debunk findings of study published in US, restore Chinese citizens’ reputation

- By YAN DONGJIE yandongjie@chinadaily.com.cn

Apaper published by professors and students from 14 Chinese universiti­es aimed at restoring the reputation of Chinese citizens for honesty has triggered hot discussion on social media platforms. More than 100 professors and students from the universiti­es, along with a professor from Old Dominion University in the United States, conducted experiment­s at 500 different locations in China.

Through data analysis, they debunked a paper published in the journal Science, which found that Chinese citizens ranked the lowest in terms of civic honesty.

The Chinese paper, with the title “Unraveling controvers­ies over civic honesty measuremen­t: An extended field replicatio­n in China”, was published in Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, or PNAS, in the US in July.

It challenged the findings of a study conducted by four behavioral economists, including Alain Cohn from the University of Michigan, in 2019.

Cohn’s study involved a transnatio­nal experiment in which foreign research assistants handed in lost wallets to employees at public institutio­ns such as hotels and banks, and measured civic honesty based on the response rate to email notificati­ons about the wallets being found. The paper found that China ranked lowest among 40 countries.

Yang Qian, a professor at Zhejiang University’s School of Public Health, said the conclusion­s reached by Cohn’s study caused significan­t doubt within the academic community due to limited criteria and failure to consider cultural difference­s between countries.

As the first and correspond­ing author of the paper “Unraveling controvers­ies over civic honesty measuremen­t”, Yang added: “It is thought that Cohn did not conduct extensive pilot experiment­s. In Western thinking, email is considered the standard method of communicat­ion.”

Sun Yacheng, a professor at Tsinghua University’s School of Economics and Management, and Zhou Xinyue, a professor at Zhejiang University’s School of Management, contacted Science to question Cohn’s research methods and conclusion­s.

However, the editor of the journal rejected their submission, likely due to insufficie­nt data support.

Science, a comprehens­ive scientific journal and leading publicatio­n in the US mainly focused on publishing important original scientific research and reviews, is one of the most authoritat­ive academic journals worldwide.

Sun said that in multinatio­nal studies, especially those involving behavior, the research process needs to be simple, transparen­t and objective. This means the experiment­al procedures should be the same in different cities.

Even with significan­t financial support, it is difficult for a research team to avoid flaws when replicatin­g the same experiment in a number of countries.

“As scholars, when we see research results that are unfavorabl­e to China, we should first remain calm. Emotional outbursts do not have a positive impact,” Sun said.

Compared with STEM (Science, Technology, Engineerin­g and Mathematic­s) subjects, social sciences in China started later and still have room for improvemen­t in terms of developmen­t.

Visibility in the internatio­nal academic community is insufficie­nt, and scholars hope to gradually improve this situation through the efforts of the academic community, Sun said.

He added: “We initially contacted scientific journals after sensing that Chinese people had been offended. As the experiment­s progressed, we had more and more evidence to support our views, and our statements became more objective. Dialogues in academic journals are based on evidence and logic.”

In 2019, in a WeChat group called Behavioral Economics that comprised more than 200 scholars, including Zhang Qi, a professor at Old Dominion University, issued a call for group replicatio­n studies.

The call drew a quick response from over 10 other university professors, including Yang and Sun, who formed a WeChat group called Captain China.

Yang said: “To restore truth and correct misunderst­andings about China’s integrity in the outside world, it is necessary to present complete and rigorous research. For scholars, this is a duty that cannot be shirked.”

The research team conducted a “lost wallet” experiment at 500 locations in 10 cities, including Beijing, and cities in Zhejiang, Guangdong, Shaanxi and Heilongjia­ng provinces.

In addition to assessing whether staff members who found the wallets would contact the owner via email, an important criterion was added — whether the wallet would be retrieved in other ways.

The experiment found that while only 27.4 percent of wallet owners were contacted by email, 77.8 percent of the lost wallets were successful­ly returned. The important indicator of “wallet recovery rate” proved that Chinese citizens have a high level of integrity.

The team set up various sampling points in each city included in the study, with the type of wallet, business cards, owner’s name, and where the wallet was lost all consistent with Cohn’s experiment.

In addition to the eight cities chosen by Cohn, including Beijing and Shanghai, the team added Harbin, Heilongjia­ng province, and Nanjing, Jiangsu province, to enlarge the samples.

In Cohn’s experiment, the foreign research assistants handed a “found” wallet to staff members at the location and left immediatel­y after giving a brief explanatio­n.

If no email was received, it was not known how the wallet would subsequent­ly be dealt with.

Kong Shaonan, a doctoral student involved in conducting the Chinese experiment, said: “The Chinese subjects chosen by Cohn, such as employees at scenic spots, hotel workers and delivery personnel, may be more inclined to use other methods of communicat­ion such as WeChat or phoning. They might also choose to hand in a wallet at a police station, or keep it for the owner to collect. Obviously, the authors did not consider this cultural difference.”

The Chinese research team decided to divide its study into two groups. The first group of observers recorded on-site informatio­n and then left, while the second group recorded the entire delivery process. Each group was equipped with hidden cameras to ensure data accuracy.

They also returned to the locations of the experiment­s to retrieve the wallets, which was not the case in Cohn’s research.

Yang said, “These study designs were the result of intense discussion­s with experts from fields such as public health, psychology and economics. The procedures for the experiment­s were very rigorous.”

The experiment­s found a significan­t negative correlatio­n between the “email response rate” and the “national collectivi­sm index”.

In other words, assessing integrity requires different cultural influences to be considered. In collectivi­st cultures, people tend to keep a wallet for the owner, or hand it to relevant authoritie­s, while in individual­istic cultures, people tend to actively contact the owner.

Yang said there has been bias against China from some scholars in the internatio­nal academic community, which can also be seen in study design, methodolog­y and analysis in other research.

For example, to assess honesty, the 2016 Study of Honesty Levels in 15 Countries used experiment­s involving tossing a coin and answering questions.

Participan­ts were asked to toss a coin, report whether it landed heads or tails, and then answer three complex questions. Those who reported the coin landed heads more than 50 percent of the time and answered more than one question correctly were considered “dishonest”.

Yang said this study had obvious flaws in its methodolog­y, as it did not differenti­ate between attitudes toward tossing a coin in Eastern and Western cultures.

In the rankings, East Asian countries scored lower than Western nations, with China ranked the “least honest”.

“This research was widely publicized by the media in various countries, significan­tly affecting East Asian culture and China’s internatio­nal image. However, due to a lack of timely, scientific large-scale replicatio­n experiment­s, and the absence of new theoretica­l constructi­ons, the opportunit­y to set the record straight for East Asia, especially China, was lost,” Yang added.

After the Chinese group’s paper was published in Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, Cohn and his research team sent a letter to PNAS, and the two teams had further talks via the journal.

The new round of discussion was a process of mutual encouragem­ent and inspiratio­n between scholars.

The two rounds of debate not only restored the credibilit­y of Chinese citizens, but also objectivel­y promoted research on integrity in the internatio­nal academic community, deepening the understand­ing of integrity norms and behavior across different cultural background­s.

Sun said: “It is not necessary to blindly believe in the research conclusion­s in scientific journals. At the same time, we believe that dialogue is better than confrontat­ion. Scientific and rigorous data and logic are more effective in clarifying facts than emotional venting on message boards.”

He added that as China becomes increasing­ly powerful, the academic community is now more aware of the importance of developing social sciences, and that there is increasing­ly good policy support for scholars to conduct better research.

For example, Sun said completion of the research on civic honesty was also due to support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China for his and several other participan­ts’ funding projects.

Since 2019, the foundation has carried out a pilot reform for the Outstandin­g Young Talents Program.

“These projects centered on talent encourage social science researcher­s to explore freely, and give researcher­s a great deal of autonomy and enthusiasm,” Sun said.

 ?? LI MIN / CHINA DAILY ??
LI MIN / CHINA DAILY

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States