Discover

SLEEP SPELUNKING

Embarking on a month-long expedition in Mammoth Cave, a pioneering physiologi­st pulled the science of sleep from obscurity.

- BY KENNETH MILLER // ILLUSTRATI­ONS BY KELLIE JAEGER

WHEN

University of Chicago physiologi­st Nathaniel Kleitman told fellow faculty members he was seeking a locale for a monthlong sleep experiment — someplace as isolated from the rhythms of day and night as the Arctic in summer — a colleague in the geology department said he knew just the spot.

Between 10 million and 15 million years ago, in what is now south-central Kentucky, trickles of groundwate­r began probing the cracks in a fossil seabed. Over the eons, the pockets grew and grew until they’d formed the most extensive cave system in North America — over 400 miles of undergroun­d chambers, canyons, tubes, sha s, and passageway­s, interwoven with Stygian rivers.

Within a few months of the suggestion, on June 4, 1938, Kleitman and a companion — a graduate student named Bruce Richardson — made the short hike to their Mammoth Cave camp, 140 feet beneath Earth’s surface. Unlike the era’s other trailblazi­ng voyages (Lindbergh leaping the Atlantic, Byrd bagging the North Pole), this one would go inward: not just below terra rma but deep into the mysteries of the human brain and body. e pair would spend 32 days and nights keeping to a 28-hour sleep-wake schedule, with their results having profound implicatio­ns for the nascent science of sleep.

Before Kleitman’s expedition, only a handful of scientists studied sleep — and not a single one did so full-time. Most saw slumber as a nonevent, a nightly state of suspended animation. Many considered it a vestige of humanity’s primitive past, which could be minimized or eliminated altogether.

Today, the quest to understand sleep — and to apply that understand­ing to our daily lives — has become a global obsession. Sleep research centers can be found at every major university. Over 2,500 sleep clinics operate across America, and 4,000 more in other countries. e World Sleep Society, which represents scientists and health care profession­als on every continent but Antarctica, boasts 14,000 members. Over the past 90-odd years, sleep has gone from an a erthought to a central element in our notions of well-being.

How sleep science evolved to its current state of sophistica­tion is thanks to the e orts of early sleep explorers, like Kleitman, who spent decades working to solve puzzles whose importance few others could grasp. When they hit dead ends, they analyzed their errors and set o in new directions. ey risked futility and failure to advance a eld that was widely regarded as scienti cally irrelevant. In the process, they changed our nights — and days — forever.

IN THE YEARS

before entering Mammoth Cave, Kleitman had set himself one central task: to transform sleep science from an obscure backwater into a thriving discipline. In the early 1930s, he had launched a series of studies aimed at scrutinizi­ng sleep with a granularit­y no one had yet attempted. He had intertwine­d goals: to analyze the rhythms of sleep and test how di erent variables in waking behavior a ected sleep rhythms. By charting factors such as body temperatur­e, body movement, and mental acuity at di erent times of day and night, and under varying conditions and routines, he hoped to map not only the contours of normal sleep but also to learn what distinguis­hed it from abnormal varieties.

Of all the variables Kleitman aimed to examine, only body temperatur­e had been studied extensivel­y. It had long been known that temperatur­es in humans uctuated by a degree or two each day, reaching a minimum sometime a er midnight and a maximum

in the a ernoon. Kleitman hypothesiz­ed that the daily cycles of both temperatur­e and sleep were dictated by patterns of habit and behavior (such as food intake, muscular exertion, and brain activity) rather than rhythms intrinsic to the brain or body. He also posited that both cycles could be changed without harm, though a person might need some time to adjust.

For a month, Kleitman tried to shi his own sleep-wake rhythm to follow a 48-hour cycle, rather than the traditiona­l 24-hour one. His temperatur­e rhythm stayed stubbornly in place. When he placed a student on a 12-hour cycle, the subject’s temperatur­e curve again refused to follow. Kleitman even tried less-radical alteration­s, placing himself and several students on cycles lasting 21 or 28 hours. Some students were able to acclimatiz­e. But Kleitman’s temperatur­e stuck to the 24-hour cycle, and he felt more irritable and exhausted with each altered day.

Why couldn’t he adjust? Were his cortical circuits less adaptable than those of other subjects, or were they more sensitive to external stimuli? It was impossible to tell without controllin­g for such stimuli, including the cues of sunset and sunrise. Hoping to eliminate those variables, Kleitman headed to Kentucky.

AT THEIR CAMP

— a vast chamber about a quarter-mile from the cave entrance, 2,500 feet long by 150 feet wide — it was imperative the researcher­s receive no sensory cues to the cycles of the 24-hour world. Richardson had also found it impossible to adjust to a 28-hour schedule in previous experiment­s. e question was whether the pair would do better undergroun­d.

Kleitman and Richardson kept to a strict regimen, attempting to sleep from 12:00 a.m. to 9:00 a.m. Sunday; 4:00 a.m. to 1:00 p.m. Monday; 8:00 a.m. to 5:00 p.m. Tuesday, and so on — nine hours in bed and 19 hours out for each cycle. Cave workers brought meals, mail, newspapers, and smoking supplies twice a day, and spirited away the contents of the chamber pots. Portable generators powered the scienti c instrument­s and gasoline lamps supplied the light, casting shadows on their specially built beds and bedside tables, stacked with equipment. To pass the time, they organized data, wrote letters, read periodical­s and books, brushed up on bridge rules, or took brief walks around the cavern. With the air a constant 54 degrees Fahrenheit, they wore overcoats, hooded sweaters, galoshes, and two layers of woolen pants to ward o hypothermi­a. Every two hours, they measured their temperatur­es, relying on alarm clocks to rouse them during sleep periods. As they lay dreaming, the motion detectors kept vigil, disgorging ticker tape into a box beside each bunk.

For ve 28-hour cycles, events in the cave went precisely as planned. But despite their desire for secrecy (Kleitman had requested that the project be carried out discreetly), the newspapers got wind of the expedition, and a swarm of reporters descended. On June 10, Mammoth Cave manager W.W. ompson sent a note with the food delivery, begging Kleitman to allow a visit from a representa­tive of the press. Kleitman consented to an interview, and a correspond­ent of the Associated Press arrived the next day.

e 700-word dispatch turned out to be a humdinger, full

It was imperative the researcher­s receive no sensory cues to the cycles of the 24-hour world.

of fascinatin­g science and colorful details. “Neither man has shaved in a week,” the correspond­ent reported. “ ere’s no sunrise or sunset to disturb them … yet they nd themselves still unconsciou­sly guided by the twenty-four-hour cycle that has ruled all their lives.” He explained the project’s rationale with admirable clarity and described the explorers’ living conditions down to the cigarette packs scattered across the breakfast table. e men assured him that their spouses had no reason to worry; the only discomfort was a slight dampness to the bedsheets. “When wives have crazy husbands, they get used to their doing things like this,” Kleitman said with a laugh, “and we feel that we may contribute something to the knowledge of men’s reactions.”

e story was picked up across the country, and more reporters came calling. When Kleitman received a request to shoot a newsreel on the last day of the expedition, the scientist again said yes. e clip, which was shown in thousands of movie theaters, opens with a title frame: STUDYING MYSTERY OF SLEEP, SCIENTISTS LIVE MONTH IN CAVE. “An alarm clock calls time on a unique experiment,” an announcer states as Richardson awakens and Kleitman extracts a length of tape from the bedside box. “Two Chicago university experts, who in the service of science have been living in the depths of Mother Earth for more than a month, end their test.” en comes a close-up of Kleitman. “We were entirely successful in our undertakin­g,” he says, “but want it known that it is in no way a stunt or an act of endurance or perseveran­ce, but a bona de scienti c experiment.”

e newsreel is in some ways misleading.

Contrary to the scientist’s declaratio­n, the experiment was not entirely successful. Once again, Kleitman had been unable to acclimatiz­e to a 28-hour schedule while his student had done so within two weeks. Kleitman’s body temperatur­e continued on its 24-hour curve, and he always began feeling sleepy around 10:00 p.m. — even when that time fell during the morning or a ernoon of the arti cial day. He later suggested that age or other variations among individual­s accounted for the di erence between his and Richardson’s adaptabili­ty. e sample size, he admitted, was too small to draw conclusion­s.

Yet such details did nothing to diminish the story’s appeal. On July 7, when the men climbed the 71 steps from the cave entrance to the surface, dozens of journalist­s, park o cials, townspeopl­e, and tourists thronged to greet them.

What struck Kleitman most sharply, he told the newspapers, was the fragrance of the forest — a pleasant shock a er more than a month with little to smell but dank stone, tobacco smoke, and bodily excretions. e clamor of the crowd must have been startling, too, as well as the commotion of bats among the oaks and hickories and the brilliance of the sky. e headlines were gleeful: SCIENTIFIC CAVEMEN EMERGE … SCIENTISTS QUIT PLAYING MOLES IN SLEEP

STUDY … OLD WORLD LOOKS STRANGE AFTER A MONTH IN CAVERN.

the cave experiment was little more than a novelty. Its repercussi­ons, however, can be felt to this day. Kleitman’s eccentric expedition would inspire a spate of experiment­s that revolution­ized our understand­ing of how sleep-wake rhythms function. More fundamenta­lly, his emergence from Mammoth Cave marked a new era: e science of sleep had come out from undergroun­d.

With one stroke, Kleitman convinced millions of people that sleep was of “major interest” — and that it was a legitimate scienti c eld. While newspapers described how his “tenthousan­d nights of scienti c experiment” had “upset many popular ideas about the technique of sleeping,” Kleitman cemented his preeminenc­e in 1939 with the publicatio­n of the rst true textbook on sleep science — a 638-page opus that would serve as the eld’s bible for decades. Kleitman spent six years writing the tome, which covered virtually every study since the 1910s, including his own.

Building on this foundation, Kleitman attributed nearly every aspect of the sleep-wake cycle to choices, behaviors, and sensory inputs rather than “some general properties of protoplasm” (that is, biological rhythms). What makes people feel the need to sleep a er an extended period of wakefulnes­s? Fatigue of the cerebral cortex — and of the muscles of the eyes, face, and neck, Kleitman said. What drives diurnal temperatur­e rhythms? Di erent levels of mental and muscular activity during sleep and waking — though the temperatur­e curve may persist through force of habit even when sleeping patterns change.

Kleitman’s text decisively establishe­d him as his eld’s foremost authority. In its pages, researcher­s could nd a set of facts, concepts, problems, and techniques to serve as points of departure for new exploratio­ns. e discipline was developing a sense of intellectu­al coherence, as well as a modicum of in uence. Yet its grasp of sleep’s underlying mechanisms — and its ability to actually improve people’s lives — remained limited.

Not until years later were Kleitman’s theories proven wrong — in part due to discoverie­s concerning the “general properties of protoplasm,” as well as new ndings in neuroanato­my and electrophy­siology. Today, scientists see sleep as part of a circadian cycle, a built-in biological clock tied to the 24-hour day. But without Kleitman’s work, blazing a trail for the thousands of sleep scientists who followed in his footsteps, this knowledge might’ve remained in the shadows — trapped in a world as deep and dark as the caves of Kentucky.

 ?? ??
 ?? ?? From the book MAPPING THE DARKNESS: The Visionary Scientists Who Unlocked the Mysteries of
Sleep by Kenneth Miller. Copyright ©
2023 by Kenneth Miller. Reprinted by permission of Hachette Books, an imprint of Perseus Books LLC., a subsidiary of Hachette Book Group Inc. New York, NY, USA. All rights reserved.
From the book MAPPING THE DARKNESS: The Visionary Scientists Who Unlocked the Mysteries of Sleep by Kenneth Miller. Copyright © 2023 by Kenneth Miller. Reprinted by permission of Hachette Books, an imprint of Perseus Books LLC., a subsidiary of Hachette Book Group Inc. New York, NY, USA. All rights reserved.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States