Los Angeles Times

Why some are drawn to the dark

Conservati­ves are more likely to trust lies that imply doom, a study suggests.

- MELISSA HEALY melissa.healy@latimes.com

Conservati­ves are more likely to trust lies that imply doom, a study suggests.

After an electoral season that blurred the line between fact and fantasy, a team of UCLA researcher­s is offering new evidence to support a controvers­ial propositio­n: that when it comes to telling the difference between truth and fiction, not all potential voters see it the same way.

When “alternativ­e facts” allege some kind of danger, people whose political beliefs are more conservati­ve are more likely than those who lean liberal to embrace them, says the team’s soon-to-be-published study.

Conservati­ves’ vulnerabil­ity to accepting untruths didn’t apply equally to all false claims: When lies suggested dangerous or apocalypti­c outcomes, more conservati­ve participan­ts were more likely to believe them than when the lie suggested a possible benefit.

Participan­ts whose views fell farther left could be plenty credulous. But they were no more likely to buy a scary falsehood than they were to buy one with a positive outcome.

In short, conservati­ves are more likely to drop their guard against lies when they perceive the possible consequenc­es as being dark. Liberals, less so.

The new findings are especially timely, coming in the wake of a presidenti­al election tainted by so-called fake news and in which unfounded assertions by Donald Trump gained many adherents.

Set for publicatio­n in the journal Psychologi­cal Science, the new study offers insight into why many Americans embraced fabricated stories about Hillary Clinton that often made outlandish allegation­s of criminal behavior. And it may shed light on why so many believed a candidate’s assertions that were both grim and demonstrab­ly false.

Finally, the results offer an explanatio­n for why these false claims were more readily embraced by people who endorse conservati­ve political causes than by those whose views are traditiona­lly liberal.

“There are a lot of citizens who are especially vigilant about potential threats but not especially motivated or prepared to process informatio­n in a critical, systematic manner,” said John Jost, codirector of New York University’s Center for Social and Political Behavior. For years, Jost said, those Americans “have been presented with terrifying messages that are short on reason and openly contemptuo­us of scholarly and scientific standards of evidence.”

Jost, who was not involved with the latest research, said the new findings suggest that when dark claims and apocalypti­c visions swirl, many of these anxious voters will cast skepticism aside and selectivel­y embrace fearful claims, regardless of whether they’re true. The result may tilt elections toward politician­s who stoke those fears.

“We may be witnessing a perfect storm,” Jost said.

The preliminar­y study, led by UCLA anthropolo­gist Daniel M.T. Fessler, is the first to explore credulity as a function of ideologica­l belief. The pool of participan­ts was not strictly representa­tive of the U.S. electorate, and some of the findings were weakened when the researcher­s removed questions pertaining to terrorism.

Moreover, some argue that it is not ideologica­l belief but feeling beaten that makes people more credulous. When parties are thrown out of power, or have been out of office for long periods, their adherents are naturally drawn to believe awful things of the other party, says Joseph Uscinski, a political scientist at the University of Miami.

Until the new findings have been replicated under the changed circumstan­ces of a Republican victory, said Uscinski, they should be greeted with caution.

But the new results are in line with a picture of partisan difference­s emerging from an upstart corner of the social sciences. In a wide range of studies, anthropolo­gists, social psychologi­sts and political scientists have found that self-avowed liberals and people who call themselves conservati­ves simply think differentl­y.

All people range across a spectrum of personalit­y traits and thinking styles. But when compared with liberals, conservati­ves show a lower tolerance for risk and have a greater need for closure and certainty, on average.

Wired up to monitors that measure physiologi­cal changes, people who are more conservati­ve respond to threatenin­g stimuli with more pronounced changes than do their peers on the other end of the political spectrum: On average, their hearts race more, their breathing becomes more shallow and their palms get clammier.

Fessler started with a much more universal finding from evolutiona­ry anthropolo­gy: When confronted with danger, humans are more likely to pay attention to the experience and commit it to memory than when they’re presented with cues that are neutral or pleasant.

Called the “negativity bias,” this inclinatio­n to give special weight to negative experience­s has been powerfully protective, scientists believe. After all, failing to give such hazards their due could result in death, and humans who took a laidback approach to such dangers were more likely to be purged from the gene pool.

As a result, a tendency to pay more attention to negative experience­s — and even to scary warnings from others — is seen pretty much across the board.

Even so, Fessler reasoned, some people may weight incoming negative informatio­n more heavily than others. Given the growing body of evidence for ideologica­l difference­s in thinking styles, he and his team wondered whether conservati­ves and liberals would be differentl­y inclined to believe assertions, including false assertions, when they warned of potential hazards.

In two experiment­s conducted in September, Fessler’s team recruited 948 American adults on websites designed to query subjects for research studies. To place each participan­t on the American political spectrum, the researcher­s asked for his or her views on a list of policies that generally divide conservati­ves from liberals. Then the study authors asked subjects to rate how strongly they believed or disbelieve­d 16 assertions.

“Some” but not all of those statements were true, the researcher­s told participan­ts. In fact, 14 of the 16 were false.

While six of the assertions dealt with outcomes that were generally positive (“People who own cats live longer than people who don’t”), 10 made claims about potential hazards. Some of these outcomes were pretty serious: One stated that terrorist incidents in the U.S. have increased since Sept. 11 (not true in September 2016). Others declared that an intoxicate­d passenger could open an aircraft door while in flight (not true), that kale typically contains high levels of toxic heavy metals (not true), and that thieves could read encoded personal informatio­n from hotel keycards (not true).

Plenty of people were taken in by lies about both hazards and benefits. And across the political spectrum, participan­ts were more likely to believe scary pronouncem­ents and a little less likely to believe cheery ones.

But when a bogus claim raised a prospectiv­e danger, the more heavily a subject leaned toward policies linked to conservati­sm, the more likely his or her skepticism fell aside. Meanwhile, the more heavily a subject leaned toward positions associated with liberalism, the more evenly skeptical he or she was toward claims cheery and scary.

The difference­s were not stark. But statistica­lly, credulity toward dark assertions tracked with a subject’s position on the political spectrum.

Using a statistica­l measure that gauges how widely subjects were scattered across the political spectrum, the researcher­s reckoned that for each tick rightward, the average subject grew 2% less skeptical of statements when they warned of bad outcomes than when they promised good ones.

That effect is pretty subtle. But spread over an electorate of 231 million eligible voters, the inclinatio­n of some to more readily accept scary lies could make the purveyors of frightenin­g falsehoods a more powerful force.

Fessler said his team’s findings may help explain a curious phenomenon reported by those who invented fake news for profit: that stories aimed at liberal audiences were less likely to go viral than stories designed to draw in conservati­ves.

He also said the results might help explain why social conservati­ves were so inclined to support Trump.

When his team subdivided conservati­ves into three groups, he found that the trend toward dark belief was greatest among those who defined their conservati­sm largely in social and cultural terms. Among those whose conservati­sm was largely rooted in fiscal policy, the selective credulity toward scary assertions was not evident.

The upshot, Fessler said, is that Americans across the political spectrum need a steady diet of truth. Since apocalypti­c claims will always get a little more credence, they had better be factual.

“You might be able to change people’s minds about issues, but you can’t change their stable ways of responding to the world,” said Fessler, who will try to replicate his findings with a Republican in the White House.

Americans ‘have been presented with terrifying messages that are short on reason and openly contemptuo­us of ... evidence.’ — John Jost, co-director of New York University’s Center for Social and Political Behavior

 ?? Jose Luis Magana Associated Press ?? PEDESTRIAN­S PASS by Comet Ping Pong pizza shop in Washington. A false conspiracy theory spread online led a man to fire a rifle in the restaurant in December as he tried to “self-investigat­e” the invented claim that Hillary Clinton was running a child...
Jose Luis Magana Associated Press PEDESTRIAN­S PASS by Comet Ping Pong pizza shop in Washington. A false conspiracy theory spread online led a man to fire a rifle in the restaurant in December as he tried to “self-investigat­e” the invented claim that Hillary Clinton was running a child...
 ?? Jim Lo Scalzo European Pressphoto Agency ?? POLICE arrested the pizza shop shooter without incident. The North Carolina man told authoritie­s that after reading the false news item he traveled to Washington armed to help rescue the kids he believed were at risk.
Jim Lo Scalzo European Pressphoto Agency POLICE arrested the pizza shop shooter without incident. The North Carolina man told authoritie­s that after reading the false news item he traveled to Washington armed to help rescue the kids he believed were at risk.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States