Los Angeles Times

Gender stereotype­s set in early

By 6, girls no longer think they’re as smart as boys — a major shift from their beliefs at age 5, study finds.

- AMINA KHAN amina.khan@latimes.com Twitter: @aminawrite

Why do so few women end up in physics, mathematic­s and other fields traditiona­lly associated with “brilliance”? Part of the answer may lie in what happens to girls by the time they’re out of kindergart­en.

A new study finds that 6-year-old girls are less likely than boys to think members of their own gender can be brilliant — and they’re more likely than boys to shy away from activities requiring that exceptiona­l intelligen­ce. That’s a serious change from their attitudes at age 5, when they’re just as likely as boys to think their own gender can be brilliant, and just as willing to take on those activities for brilliant children.

The results, described in the journal Science, show how early these gender stereotype­s begin to affect the self-perception and behavior of girls — which may limit their aspiration­s and careers into adulthood.

“If we want to change young people’s minds and make things more equitable for girls, we really need to know when this problemati­c stereotype first emerges, and then we know when to intervene to avoid these negative consequenc­es on girls’ educationa­l decisions and their future career choices,” said lead author Lin Bian, a graduate student in psychology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

The stereotype that men are better at math and science is a pervasive one, difficult to dislodge even at the highest echelons of higher education. In 2005, Harvard President Lawrence Summers stirred up controvers­y during a speech in which he said that women were underrepre­sented in the sciences in part because of “issues of intrinsic aptitude.”

And a study in the journal PLOS One last year found that men in collegelev­el biology classes consistent­ly overestima­ted the performanc­e of their male peers and underestim­ated the performanc­e of their female classmates. (The women, by the way, evaluated their fellow students far more accurately in terms of performanc­e, apparently without regard to gender.)

These ideas aren’t just a reflection of perceived difference­s in gender, according to Bian and her colleagues. They’re also a reflection of perceived intelligen­ce.

“Popular beliefs about ability associate not only specific cognitive processes (e.g., mathematic­al reasoning) with a particular gender but also the overall amount of cognitive ability,” the study authors wrote. “It is commonly assumed that high-level cognitive ability (brilliance, genius, giftedness, etc.) is present more often in men than in women. This ‘brilliance = males’ stereotype has been invoked to explain the gender gaps in many prestigiou­s occupation­s.”

These stereotype­s may have serious consequenc­es for young women in college and their future careers. For example, the authors point out, previous research has shown that the idea that men are better than women at math actually impairs women’s performanc­e and undermines their interest in math-related fields.

But how far back does the stereotype that men are more likely to be “brilliant” start to shape the behaviors of young men and women? To find out, the scientists set up a series of experiment­s to test the gender perception­s of 5-, 6- and 7-year-olds.

In one experiment, 96 children were told a story about a person who was “really, really smart” — a kid-friendly version of the word “brilliant” — but they weren’t told the person’s gender. The children were then asked to guess which of four adults, two men and two women, that person was. They were also shown pairs of adults — both women, both men, or a man and a woman — and asked to pick which adult in each pair was “really, really smart.” Finally, they completed puzzles in which they had to associate objects (such as a hammer) or attributes (including “smart”) with pictures of men and women.

The scientists found that from ages 5 to 7, children’s perception­s of brilliance go through fairly dramatic changes. At 5, boys and girls both associated brilliance with their own gender on roughly the same level. But by 6 and 7, girls were significan­tly less likely than boys to associate brilliance with their own gender.

The second experiment replicated the first one — except with 144 children who were asked to also rate kids, not just adults, in the tasks — and the results still held.

Strangely, when asked who got the best grades in school, the older girls were just as likely as the younger ones to pick girls — and in fact, older girls were more likely than older boys to choose their own gender as getting the best grades.

This was consistent with the reality that girls do get better grades than boys at that age, the authors wrote.

“Neverthele­ss, there was no significan­t correlatio­n between girls’ perception­s of school achievemen­t and their perception­s of brilliance,” the authors wrote. “Thus, girls’ ideas about who is brilliant are not rooted in their perception­s of who performs well in school.”

The authors also tested whether these beliefs about gender and brilliance affected girls’ interests. The researcher­s had 64 6- and 7-year-olds play two games — one for “really, really smart” (i.e. “brilliant”) children and one for “children who try really, really hard.” Girls seemed drawn to the “hard-working” game about as much as boys — but they were significan­tly less interested than boys were in the game for brilliant kids.

When the researcher­s repeated the study with 96 5- and 6-year-olds, however, they found no significan­t difference­s in the 5-year-old girls’ and boys’ interest. (The 6-year-old girls, on the other hand, clearly had begun to lose their enthusiasm for the “smart” games.)

“The present results suggest a sobering conclusion: Many children assimilate the idea that brilliance is a male quality at a young age,” the authors wrote. “This stereotype begins to shape children’s interests as soon as it is acquired and is thus likely to narrow the range of careers they will one day contemplat­e.”

That this shift occurs around age 5 may have to do with children entering more formal school scenarios, where they’re exposed to many other people (both children and adults), said Yarrow Dunham, a developmen­tal psychologi­st at Yale University who was not involved in the study.

It’s unclear which of the myriad social influences happening at the time could be contributi­ng to girls’ shift in self-perception, he added. It’s possible, for example, that teachers might be unconsciou­sly reinforcin­g stereotype­s by how they respond to boys and girls in the classroom. Or perhaps the exposure to history books that mostly feature men causes children to assume that women are largely absent because they were less intelligen­t (rather than because of laws and social mores that for centuries treated women as second-class citizens). For now, the complex causes of these stereotype­s remain an open question.

“It is dishearten­ing and it really calls for some thought about identifyin­g what are the causal variables,” Dunham said. “Is it teachers? Is it the kind of historical materials that they’re exposed to? Because some of those will be easier fixes than others. And so identifyin­g which is the main causal variable seems pretty important.”

Identifyin­g those variables will be the first step to figuring out how to weed out these stereotype­s as soon as they take root.

 ?? Katie Falkenberg For The Times ?? GENDER STEREOTYPE­S begin to affect girls’ self-perception and behavior at age 6, a study found, adding that this could limit their aspiration­s and careers into adulthood. Above, a rover drives over children during a 2012 open house at the Jet...
Katie Falkenberg For The Times GENDER STEREOTYPE­S begin to affect girls’ self-perception and behavior at age 6, a study found, adding that this could limit their aspiration­s and careers into adulthood. Above, a rover drives over children during a 2012 open house at the Jet...

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States