Los Angeles Times

OUR UNDERRATED NOSES

We finally get credit for a good sense of smell

- DEBORAH NETBURN deborah.netburn @latimes.com Twitter: @DeborahNet­burn

For far too long, we humans have suffered from an olfactory inferiorit­y complex.

We’ve been led to believe that we have a sadly deficient sense of smell compared with our mammalian cousins such as rodents and dogs.

What does the world smell like to a bloodhound? we might wonder. What scents — glorious or gross — can a twitchy little mouse nose detect that are passing right by us?

You can stop wondering, because it turns out our sense of smell is not so bad after all.

In fact, there is no evidence that our sniffing abilities are any worse than those of other mammals, according to a study published this week in Science. Absolutely none. The title of the study says it all: “Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth.”

“This paper sweeps away a few centuries of insecurity about our capacity to smell,” said Leslie Vosshall, who studies olfaction at Rockefelle­r University in New York City.

Vosshall, who was not involved in the research, added: “This work is important both for the field and as a cautionary tale for scientists to question everything — and for all of us to take some time to smell the roses.”

The paper was written by John McGann, a sensory neuroscien­tist at Rutgers University in New Jersey. McGann has spent most of his career studying the olfactory system in mice, but recently he decided to see whether some of the work he was doing with rodents would apply to humans.

In one of the first experiment­s, he wanted to test how people learn to distinguis­h between two similar odors.

But there was a hitch: Finding two odors that the human subjects couldn’t tell apart proved more challengin­g than McGann and his students expected.

Even when they used two odors that mice consistent­ly struggle to differenti­ate, humans had no trouble figuring out that they were not the same, indicating that at least in some cases, our olfactory abilities trump those of mice.

“I’d always kind of known that the human sense of smell was better than most people give it credit for, but it was striking to us how good it was,” McGann said.

And so his curiosity was piqued: Where did the idea that people have shoddy sniffers originate? And what proof was there that it was true?

To find out, he launched an investigat­ion that led him to 150-year-old medical texts, the early work of Sigmund Freud and some contempora­ry scientific studies.

McGann traced the idea that humans are bad smellers back to Paul Broca, a prominent 19th century neurosurge­on and anthropolo­gist from France.

Broca’s work on comparativ­e neurobiolo­gy led him to believe that our ability to exercise free will came at the expense of being able to smell as well as other animals.

He came to this conclusion in part by observing that the region of the brain he identified with rational thought — the frontal lobe of the cerebral cortex — is relatively enlarged in humans compared with other mammals. He also noted that the two areas of the brain that process smells — known as the olfactory bulbs — are relatively smaller in humans than in other mammals.

In 1879, Broca divided animals into two groups. Most were classified as osmatics, or those that use smell as their principal sense. Those that didn’t were non-osmatics.

Broca put humans in the non-osmatic category — but not because we can’t smell well. Rather, it was because he believed we are able to choose whether to respond to smelly stimuli.

In 1890, another researcher, William Turner, subdivided Broca’s nonosmatic group into two additional categories — species that can’t smell at all (such as whales and dolphins) and those he deemed feeble smellers (humans and apes).

But McGann emphasized that at the time, Turner had no evidence that humans were actually any worse at smelling than species in the osmatic group.

It didn’t matter. By then, the idea that humans are deficient in the olfactory department had begun to stick.

In 1924, American neurologis­t C.J. Herrick wrote that the olfactory organs of humans are “greatly reduced, almost vestigial.”

Herrick also claimed that we could not even begin to imagine what the world must smell like to other animals. “The enormously larger apparatus of most other mammals gives them powers far beyond our comprehens­ion,” he wrote.

British surgeon Sir Victor Negus piled on in 1958, writing: “The human mind is an inadequate agent with which to study olfaction for the reason that in Man the sense of smell is relatively feeble and not of great significan­ce.”

Even Freud jumped on the bandwagon. He claimed that smell is “usually atrophied” in humans and that people who take pleasure in scents are more animalisti­c, and therefore more likely to suffer from sexual disorders.

To be fair, there is some biological evidence that might, at first glance, bolster the argument that our sense of smell doesn’t stack up.

For example, our olfactory bulbs take up much less brain space than those of mice. These smelling centers compose 0.01% of the human brain by volume, compared with 2% of the mouse brain.

But that argument is not entirely convincing, McGann wrote. Because our brains are much larger than mouse brains, the human olfactory bulb is more than four times larger than its murine counterpar­t.

Another claim of our poor smelling ability came more recently in the form of genetic analysis. Scientists have determined that humans have approximat­ely 400 genes activated by odors, compared with about 1,000 in mice. This finding was immediatel­y used to confirm the comparativ­ely limited olfactory ability in primates, McGann wrote, “although no actual sensory testing was performed.”

Meanwhile, a few recent studies have started to make the case that the human sense of smell is actually very robust.

In 2014, Vosshall’s lab revealed that humans could distinguis­h 1 trillion distinct smells. Before that, the prevailing wisdom held that humans were capable of smelling only 10,000 odors. That idea was first floated in an influentia­l 1927 study by chemical engineer Ernest C. Crocker, but was never tested.

Other studies have shown that humans are capable of tracking scent trails outdoors, similar to dogs. They’ve also found that humans can be more sensitive to some smells than rats and monkeys, and worse at detecting other smells.

McGann noted that if you count the number of neurons in the olfactory bulbs of a variety of animals, you’ll find that human males have the fewest, then mice, hamsters, guinea pigs, human females, macaques, monkeys and, finally, rats.

Overall, McGann concluded that our sense of smell is excellent and that humans with intact olfactory systems can detect virtually all volatile chemicals larger than an atom or two.

In fact, he said, scientists are working to identify the few odorants we can’t smell.

So hopefully all this will make you feel better about your nose — though it may not inspire much confidence in sensory science.

“How could so many scientists have shrugged and went along with it, even though there was no actual testing?” McGann said. “That’s what really bothers me.”

 ?? Ricardo DeAratanha Los Angeles Times ?? HUMANS ARE capable of distinguis­hing about 1 trillion smells. It turns out mice have fewer olfactory bulb neurons than women, but more than men.
Ricardo DeAratanha Los Angeles Times HUMANS ARE capable of distinguis­hing about 1 trillion smells. It turns out mice have fewer olfactory bulb neurons than women, but more than men.
 ?? Tiripero Getty Images ??
Tiripero Getty Images

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States