Los Angeles Times

Marmosets prove to be selfless

The monkeys show themselves to be generous, giving in private, study reveals.

- MELISSA HEALY melissa.healy@latimes.com Twitter: @LATMelissa­Healy

Marmoset monkeys exist on a branch of the evolutiona­ry tree that is distinct from the one that led to humans. But these fellow primates consistent­ly astonish researcher­s with social behavior that seems, well, pretty highly evolved.

Marmosets engage in rigorously polite patterns of communicat­ion and do not talk over or interrupt one another. Though territoria­l, they are so inquisitiv­e that they will watch videos of marmosets they do not know and learn from them.

Marmosets’ social organizati­on and child-rearing practices could have been the model for the phrase “It takes a village.” A dominant male and female breed, and their babies are meticulous­ly looked after by extended family members who then aren’t free to breed themselves.

A new study further burnishes the marmoset’s reputation for admirable community values. Researcher­s report that these caregivers share their food more generously with little ones in private than when they’re surrounded by the watchful eyes of other community members.

Such seemingly selfless behavior makes the common marmoset a particular puzzle for researcher­s keen to explain the already perplexing phenomenon of altruism in humans and a few other higher-order primates.

After all, in stark evolutiona­ry terms, kindness, generosity and self-sacrifice make no sense. When an individual’s survival and the propagatio­n of his genes arguably depend on his hoarding every advantage for himself, why on earth would he forfeit anything to benefit a fellow member of his species?

For marmosets, it’s hard enough to explain the willingnes­s of some family members to delay or forgo breeding to help out with a relative’s offspring. But to share food with those babies when no one’s looking? That just defies all expectatio­ns.

In complex societies where individual­s band together for mutual protection, researcher­s have come up with a few widely embraced explanatio­ns for altruistic behavior.

Boosting the prospects of a relative’s child — kinship selection — does help propagate at least a few of an individual’s genes and thus seems a good general explanatio­n for self-sacrificin­g behavior. But specific acts, like sharing a delicious cricket with a begging baby marmoset, seem to need more specific explanatio­n.

One possibilit­y is that an individual practices generosity as a means of enhancing his status among peers. By demonstrat­ing that he is so well endowed with material goods that he can give some away, this do-gooder enhances his prestige within the group. That, in turn, may appeal to prospectiv­e mates.

The other explanatio­n for charitable behavior posits that kindnesses extended to others are simply the dues of group membership, which offers some future promise of a chance to mate. Failure to share would result in expulsion from the group and a loss of prospectiv­e partners. Scientists call this the “pay to stay” model.

Importantl­y, for both of these models to work, acts of kindness must have an audience. That suggests you would see more sharing in group settings; away from judging eyes, a caregiver might be more inclined to keep food for himself or herself.

And yet, in 2,581 trials conducted with 31 adult and 14 baby marmosets, the opposite appeared to be true.

Anthropolo­gists from the University of Zurich carefully documented how often, in groups and in conditions that found caregiver and baby separated from the crowd, an adult would share his or her cricket.

When alone with a baby begging for a taste, adult marmosets shared their cricket 85% of the time. When in a group, caregivers offered up their cricket 67% of the time.

“Our results show that helping in common marmosets is not driven by reputation management or punishment avoidance,” the study authors reported. “Rather, it is driven by an intrinsic motivation to help that is more strongly expressed when individual­s are alone with offspring.”

What motivated these acts of kindness? The authors offered a human parallel as a possibilit­y.

“The marmoset pattern of results calls to mind the so-called diffusion of responsibi­lity effect, which is well establishe­d in humans,” the authors write. “People are less likely to help in the presence of a larger number of bystanders, but feel more responsibl­e when alone with a needy individual.”

Whether that pattern fits will require more research, not only in marmosets but with chimpanzee­s and capuchin monkeys, whose breeding patterns are far less cooperativ­e.

The study was published last month in the journal Biology Letters.

 ?? Adrian Dennis AFP/Getty Images ?? MARMOSETS at London’s Whipsnade Zoo in 2013. In 2,581 trials conducted with 31 adult and 14 baby marmosets, adult marmosets alone with babies shared crickets 85% of the time, University of Zurich anthropolo­gists found. In a group setting, the rate was...
Adrian Dennis AFP/Getty Images MARMOSETS at London’s Whipsnade Zoo in 2013. In 2,581 trials conducted with 31 adult and 14 baby marmosets, adult marmosets alone with babies shared crickets 85% of the time, University of Zurich anthropolo­gists found. In a group setting, the rate was...

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States