Los Angeles Times

How falsities can overpower truth

Rumors and inaccurate threats aren’t helping.

- By Melissa Healy

The plot had all the elements of a big-budget thriller: Put an untested gene therapy technology in the hands of unscrupulo­us scientists. Make them soldiers of a greedy pharmaceut­ical company. Then give them the protection of a secretive and authoritar­ian government that will stop at nothing to achieve world domination.

They will, of course, unleash a deadly new virus on the world.

On Twitter last week, more than 2.5 million followers of the financial blog ZeroHedge saw this plot spun as the origin story of the new coronaviru­s from China that is spreading across the globe.

The rumor circulated briefly on several social media platforms before Twitter shut down the ZeroHedge account for violating its rules against “deceptive activity that misleads others.” But by then, it had been posted, shared or commented upon more than 9,000 times, according to the Digital Forensic Research Lab.

Stamping out falsehoods about the coronaviru­s will require much more than blocking a Twitter account. Indeed, thanks to the way we are wired to process informatio­n about new and mysterious threats, it may be all but impossible, experts say.

“Misinforma­tion is a worrisome consequenc­e of any emerging epidemic,” said Dartmouth College political scientist Brendan Nyhan, who studies conspiracy theories and those who believe them. “But the assumption that facts and science alone are going to be decisive in countering misinforma­tion is wrong, because they often aren’t.”

Researcher­s from a variety of discipline­s have examined why people believe things that have been discredite­d or debunked. Their efforts have led them to a worrying surmise: Most of us are primed to give some credence to fibs we see or hear. And once we have done so, we’re loath to update our beliefs, even when offered alternativ­es that are true.

Indeed, efforts aimed at debunking false informatio­n can wind up reinforcin­g them instead.

That presents a serious challenge to the public health officials working to persuade panicked members of the public to remain calm.

Some of the untruths about the coronaviru­s appear motivated by commercial interest. Others seem driven by ethnic suspicion and xenophobia. Still others have speculated that the coronaviru­s is a bioweapon developed by China to kill Uighur Muslims, or by the United States to destroy the Chinese economy.

The problem with conspiracy theories is that they often seem to have some whiff of truth. That makes them just plausible enough to be credible.

Some rumors connect the virus to unfounded yet well-establishe­d beliefs, such as those linking vaccines to autism and geneticall­y modified foods to health risks. In social media communitie­s devoted to those beliefs, speculatio­ns about the coronaviru­s circulate daily, said Joshua Introne, a computer scientist at Syracuse University who studies the evolution of conspiracy theories online.

“We like things that support what we believe,” Introne said. Embracing those stories tends both to deepen our conviction­s and to prompt us to share them with others of like mind, he added.

In many ways, misinforma­tion has a built-in advantage over the truth.

A person doesn’t have to believe every aspect of a conspiracy theory to keep it going. If just one component of the story jibes with her beliefs — a suspicion of China, say, or a conviction that drug companies would do anything for money — that could be enough to make her want to share the story, and perhaps suggest some further plot twist.

The more tropes that can be woven into a conspiracy theory, the more chances it has to gain a following. The resulting “multiverse” of believers makes it hard to destroy, Introne said.

“There’s just too much there,” he said.

Health officials have a natural instinct to counter this misinforma­tion with facts. But research shows how that can backfire.

Correcting misinforma­tion may work briefly, but the passage of time can taint our memories. Sometimes, all we take away from the correction is that there’s bogus informatio­n out there, so we’re skeptical when presented with facts that are true.

Sometimes the effort to correct misinforma­tion involves repeating the lie. That repetition seems to establish it in our memories more firmly than the truth, causing us to recall it better and believe it more. Psychologi­sts call this the “illusory truth effect.”

Consider the attempt by health authoritie­s in Brazil to set the record straight about the Zika virus, which took the country by storm in 2015. Most infections resulted in nothing more than mild illnesses, but pregnant women who contracted the virus found themselves at greater risk of suffering miscarriag­es or giving birth to babies with microcepha­ly and other birth defects.

The virus is spread by mosquitoes, and the public was urged to wear insect repellent and take other protective measures. Officials made their case by sharing scientific­ally accurate informatio­n about the virus. Yet their efforts caused people to doubt facts that had firm scientific grounding, and false beliefs were still flourishin­g two years after the outbreak began.

Close to two-thirds of Brazilians believed an unfounded claim that Zika was being spread by geneticall­y modified mosquitoes, according to a study published last month in the journal Science Advances. More than half incorrectl­y attributed the increased prevalence of microcepha­ly in newborns to mosquitoki­lling larvicides. And more than half believed the DTaP vaccine contribute­d to the uptick in babies born with microcepha­ly.

Once a participan­t was prompted to doubt the veracity of some of his Zika-related beliefs, he became more skeptical of any incoming informatio­n about the virus, the researcher­s found.

When you warn people that there is fake news out there, “they may apply it in an indiscrimi­nate way,” said Nyhan, who worked on the study. “People may doubt all sorts of legitimate informatio­n.”

This effect was widespread, and it was evident among respondent­s whether or not they were inclined toward believing conspiracy theories.

Researcher­s also know there’s nothing like the allure of something new. The truth doesn’t change, but new falsehoods spring up every day.

After Twitter banned ZeroHedge, traffic on the site appears to have spiked, said Emerson Brooking, a resident fellow at the Digital Forensic Research Lab. That, he said, is a common short-term reaction to a sensationa­l act of conspiracy­mongering.

The proliferat­ion of false and misleading stories about the coronaviru­s “has contribute­d to a diminished trust among people in anything they read about the crisis,” including true and well-sourced informatio­n, Brooking said.

Nyhan is working on communicat­ion strategies to deal with this problem. In experiment­s, he and a colleague found that instead of just correcting false informatio­n, it’s much more effective to replace it.

“A causal explanatio­n for an unexplaine­d event is significan­tly more effective than a denial,” they reported in the Journal of Experiment­al Political Science.

It may not be as compelling a tale, but if it’s simple and straightfo­rward, it can fill a gap left by the misinforma­tion, Nyhan said. It’s likely to work best if it comes from a trusted intermedia­ry, such as a barber, pastor or doctor. And when they present the claim being corrected, they should give fair warning that it is false or misleading.

With the coronaviru­s still taking its greatest toll in Asia, Americans’ willingnes­s to believe untruths about the virus has not reached crisis proportion­s. But if viral transmissi­on within the United States begins, experts said, the tide of misinforma­tion will rise. And our confidence in what we know to be true — and in what we’re told is accurate — will be put to the test.

“We have lost our gatekeeper­s, and we have nothing to replace them,” Introne said. “We’ve got to figure this out.”

 ?? Frederic J. Brown AFP/Getty Images ?? FEARS ABOUT coronaviru­s can muddle truth with rumors. Above, a news alert from Alhambra schools.
Frederic J. Brown AFP/Getty Images FEARS ABOUT coronaviru­s can muddle truth with rumors. Above, a news alert from Alhambra schools.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States