Los Angeles Times

Study finds disparity in how police talk to motorists

Officers use friendlier tone when speaking to white men than Black peers, research shows.

- By Amina Khan

Scientists who analyzed the body-camera footage from more than 100 police officers have found a subtle but clear pattern: During traffic stops, officers spoke to Black men in a less respectful and less friendly tone than they did to white men.

This disparity in treatment is not only real but may also help to fuel a cycle of mistrust between police and the Black community, the researcher­s reported this week in the Journal of Personalit­y and Social Psychology.

Law enforcemen­t experts agreed.

“It really reflects the amount of work that law enforcemen­t needs to continue to do,” said Diane Goldstein, executive director of the nonprofit Law Enforcemen­t Action Partnershi­p. “We have to recognize the problem, we have to acknowledg­e our unconsciou­s biases in these types of situations, and we have to acknowledg­e the role that race plays in the criminal justice system.”

The highly publicized deaths of Black men, women and children at the hands of police in recent years have focused the public’s attention on the deadly consequenc­es of such discrimina­tion, and scientists have been busy quantifyin­g the myriad ways that this disparity in treatment manifests.

A 2019 paper, for example, found that Black men were 2 ½ times more likely than white men to be killed by police. Additional investigat­ions around the U.S. have found that Black residents suffer a disproport­ionate share of use-of-force incidents relative to their share of the population.

Much of what’s known about disparitie­s comes from administra­tive records, such as reports of police stops, said Nicholas Camp, a social psychologi­st at the University of Michigan and lead author of the new study.

Take a 2020 analysis of 95 million traffic stops that found Black Americans are more likely to be pulled over than white drivers and more likely to be searched, even though they’re less likely than their white peers to be carrying illegal contraband.

More than 60 million Americans are thought to make contact with law enforcemen­t each year, which means that even small difference­s in treatment can add up to a big effect. But there’s a limit to what researcher­s can learn about these difference­s, because police stop reports typically offer little insight into the human-to-human interactio­n that actually took place.

“Every interactio­n that people have with law enforcemen­t [is] really consequent­ial for building or eroding trust, but we don’t really know what police officers actually do when they’re interactin­g with the public,” Camp said. They can’t reveal whether the officers treated community members with respect, or with contempt.

Body-worn cameras have helped fill in this gap. The cameras, which have become increasing­ly common in police department­s across the U.S. as a way to increase transparen­cy and accountabi­lity, turned out to be a rich source of data on the daily interactio­ns between officers and residents.

In previous research published in the Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, Camp and his colleagues analyzed more than 100 hours of police body-cam footage and concluded that officers’ language was less respectful toward Black residents than their white peers.

Compared with white residents, Black community members were 57% less likely to hear the officer use words such as “sir,” “ma’am” and “thank you” and 61% more likely to hear words such as “dude” and “bro” and commands such as “hands on the wheel.”

For the new paper, Camp and his colleagues focused not on what officers said but on how they said it.

The scientists analyzed hundreds of audio clips — each roughly 10 seconds long — from routine traffic stops of Black or white men. The researcher­s filtered out the high frequencie­s of the sound clips, which essentiall­y rendered the clips unintellig­ible but left the tone of voice intact. They also masked the drivers’ voices with “brown noise,” so that anyone hearing the clip would not be able to guess the motorists’ race.

The researcher­s then asked more than 400 people — a diverse group of white, Latino, Asian and Black volunteers — to listen to the clips and rate the officers’ tone of voice.

Across the board, clips of officers speaking to Black men got lower marks for friendline­ss, respectful­ness and ease than those of officers speaking to white men — even though the listeners were not aware of the drivers’ race.

On a scale of 1 to 6, the average score of officer tone toward white drivers was 3.72 (slightly positive) while the average score toward black drivers was 3.5 (neither positive nor negative).

“They’re not huge, but that’s not the point,” said Tracey Meares, a law professor at Yale University who specialize­s in citizen perception of police and was not involved in the study.

“The point is that there are difference­s that can be detected.”

The gap remained even when researcher­s controlled for a range of factors, including the driver’s age and sex, the officer’s race and gender, whether a citation was issued or a search conducted, and the listener’s age, political orientatio­n, race and gender.

The scientists also found that listeners who said they’d been treated unfairly by police tended to rate the officers’ tone as more negative, less respectful and less friendly than listeners who said they’d been treated more fairly.

Investigat­ing this issue further, the researcher­s asked some participan­ts to listen to either the 40 most positively rated or the 40 most negatively rated audio clips, and then asked them to describe a hypothetic­al officer in that department.

Those who listened to the more negative clips described an officer who was more likely to treat citizens rudely or to be accused of racial profiling — suggesting that an officer’s negative tone can have an effect on a person’s perception of the police in general.

In another experiment, the researcher­s asked participan­ts to listen to 20 randomly selected audio clips of police stops. Participan­ts who listened to clips of Black drivers being stopped were less likely to feel that officers would treat them with fairness in a similar situation — a sign that individual officers can undermine trust in the institutio­n as a whole.

“This is a [really] amazing study,” said Meares, who praised the researcher­s’ methods for removing the specific meaning in the voices while leaving their tone intact. “I wish I did this study.”

Goldstein, who spent more than two decades in the Redondo Beach Police Department, said she was struck by the way listeners’ prior experience with police played a role in their positive or negative impression of the audio clips.

She pointed out that she and other officers are trained in restorativ­e justice, setting respectful patterns and de-escalating situations. And yet, “the perception of our interactio­n with this citizen can be completely ruined by that one police officer who mistreated someone.”

Goldstein was quick to point out that the disparitie­s in vocal tone are symptomati­c of a larger problem in the relationsh­ip between police and the communitie­s they serve.

“Think about this,” she said. “Lack of trust means victims, survivors, witnesses won’t talk to us — which makes it harder to do our job, which makes our communitie­s less safe, for not just our community members but also for our police officers.”

Ultimately, the findings highlight just how challengin­g it can be to institute change in behaviors and practices that are as widespread as they are deeply ingrained — and not just in police department­s.

“A lot of this probably is unconsciou­s,” Meares said. “It says to me that it’s work that needs to be done by everyone — and interventi­ons, if you want to call it that, need to be societywid­e. Because why would we think that the officers in this study are fundamenta­lly different from anyone else?”

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States