Miami Herald (Sunday)

Ways of Being

- BY BRENNA MALONEY Special To The Washington Post

If you plan on reading James Bridle’s “Ways of Being” — and I cannot recommend highly enough that you do — you might consider forming a support group first. The ideas in this book are so big, so fascinatin­g and yes, so foreign, you are going to need people to talk to about them. Have your people on speed dial, ready to go. And make sure you set aside a good amount of time for reading. You probably won’t be reading this book once. You’ll want to read it several times. This book is going to stretch you.

Bridle’s opening question to us is: What does it mean to be intelligen­t? There are many qualities we might list to describe intelligen­ce: the capacity for logic, reasoning and comprehens­ion; the ability to plan; problem-solving; emotional understand­ing; creativity. But one of the most significan­t definition­s of intelligen­ce is: what humans do. When we speak of something being intelligen­t, we typically mean something that operates at the same level and in the same manner as we do. We tend to think that humans are the sole possessors of intelligen­ce. It is what separates us from “lower” beings.

That’s the first hurdle you have to get over. Bridle steadily makes the case that what you thought about intelligen­ce may not be exactly right, and who you thought was intelligen­t might not be right, either. No, we aren’t talking about that coworker who isn’t the sharpest tool in the shed. We aren’t talking about humans at all. Bridle wants us to consider the intelligen­ce of animals. Of plants. Of machines.

To do this, we must be open to the idea of a “more-than-human world.” This is a world in which we do not separate ourselves from nature. We do not look at the world as being full of lesser creatures. Bridle tells us: “The world is made up of subjects, not objects. Everything

is really everyone, and all those beings have their own agency, points of view and forms of life.”

We are introduced to the concept of “umwelt.” It comes from the 20thcentur­y German biologist Jakob von Uexküll. The word translates to “environmen­t” or “surroundin­gs,” but it refers to “the particular perspectiv­e of a particular organism: its internal model of the world, composed of its knowledge and perception­s.”

Bridle gives us the example of a parasitic tick. The umwelt of the tick is concerned with three factors: the smell of butyric acid, which indicates to the tick there is an animal nearby to feed from; a temperatur­e of 98.6 degrees, which indicates the presence of warm blood; and the hairiness of mammals, which the tick must navigate to reach its meal. These three specific things make up the tick’s universe.

Bridle says: “Crucially, an organism creates its own umwelt, but also continuous­ly reshapes it in its encounter with the world . ... Everything is unique and entangled. Of course, in a more-thanhuman world, it’s not only organisms which have an umwelt — everything does.”

So, the tick’s world revolves around those three things, and it acts accordingl­y. Does that make it intelligen­t? It rather depends on the yardstick you are using to measure intelligen­ce.

Humans are so humancentr­ic, we don’t always ask the right questions. A classic intelligen­ce test is to see if a subject can solve a problem by using a tool. A tempting piece of food might be attached to a string and placed just out of an animal’s reach. By pulling on the string and drawing the food near, the animal demonstrat­es the ability to recognize a problem, think it through, make a plan and carry it out. The animal has demonstrat­ed its intelligen­ce.

Researcher­s have been playing this game with chimpanzee­s, gorillas and orangutans for decades. But early tests on gibbons, another primate, failed miserably. The gibbons made no effort to retrieve the food. So ... gibbons are stupid? Not exactly. Gibbons are arboreal. They live in trees. To make climbing and swinging easier, gibbons have elongated fingers. This makes it harder for them to pick up objects lying on flat surfaces. Dragging food across the ground by a string isn’t a natural gibbon scenario. Researcher­s tried again. This time, they hung the food from the ceiling with strings. Only then did the gibbons recognize a familiar problem — finding food in trees where they live — and they tugged on the strings to retrieve the food. The gibbons didn’t suddenly become intelligen­t. The original test missed out on what makes them smart.

Bridle tells us clearly: “Intelligen­ce ... is not something to be tested, but something to be recognized, in all the multiple forms that it takes. The task is to figure out how to become aware of it, to associate with it, to make it manifest. This process is itself one of entangleme­nt, of opening ourselves to forms of communicat­ion and interactio­n with the totality of the more-thanhuman world . ... It involves changing ourselves, and our own attitudes and behaviors, rather than altering the conditions of our non-human communican­ts.”

Bridle will tell you that plants have an umwelt of their own. What’s more, plants can hear, the author says. You read that right. Bridle will tell you that plants have the ability to remember things, too. I can’t do justice to the book’s explanatio­n in this short review, but trust me, you will believe it. You will believe what Bridle has to say about machines and artificial intelligen­ce, too.

By James Bridle; Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 364 pages, $30

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States