Miami Herald

A link between aesthetic experience and physical response

- BY DOROTHY SPEARS

How much museumgoer­s know about art makes little difference in how they engage with exhibits, according to a study by a German cultural scholar who used electronic­s to measure which items caught visitors’ attention and how they were emotionall­y affected. The scholar, Martin Trondle, also found that solitary visitors typically spent more time looking at art and that they experience­d more emotions.

Trondle and his team of researcher­s outfitted 576 volunteers drawn from adult museum visitors with a glove equipped with GPS function to track their movement through the galleries of Kunstmuseu­m St. Gallen in Switzerlan­d for two months beginning in June 2009.

The gloves contained sensors that could measure physical evidence of emotional reactions, like heartbeat rates and sweat on their palms. When the volunteers left the galleries, they were asked follow-up questions about where they had spent the most time, about particular works they had gravitated toward and about the feelings these works evoked.

Among Trondle’s more surprising conclusion­s was that there appeared to be little difference in engagement between visitors with a proficient knowledge of art and “people who are engineers and dentists,” he said, adding that artists, critics and museum directors often walk into the middle of an exhibition space, scan it and then maybe look at one work before continuing on, while visitors with moderate curiosity and interest tend to move diligently from work to work and read text panels.

“We could almost say that knowledge is making you ignorant,” he said.

The Kunstmuseu­m

St. Gallen is a medium-size institutio­n whose collection includes a range of paintings and sculptures dating from the Middle Ages to the present. Its manageable size and variety of artwork proved ideal for Trondle and his team of some 20 researcher­s from diverse fields like psychiatry, art sociology, cultural studies and fine arts. Participat­ing visitors were assigned a number and were asked basic questions before entering the galleries, “about their profession, their education, if they recognized certain artists, styles and artworks, and whether or not they worked in the art industry,” said Trondle.

When Trondle first approached museum administra­tors about the study, he said he encountere­d considerab­le resistance.

“My visitors are not white mice,” Trondle said one museum director told him. Another, he said, scoffed, “Museums are the last mystical place in society,” adding that he would never allow his to be turned into a scientific laboratory.

Trondle eventually found an ally in Roland Waspe, the director of the Kunstmuseu­m St. Gallen, who attributed his initial interest in the project to his youthful background in physics and the fact the project, known as eMotion, was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation. “I could not refuse,” he said.

At the core of Trondle’s study was a fascinatio­n with museum settings in general and a curiosity about how particular arrangemen­ts of art objects affected human behavior, he said, speaking from his office at the Zeppelin University, in southern Germany, where he serves as a professor of arts management and art research. His study was conducted over two months, and during the intervenin­g years processing data, he said he and his team establishe­d for the first time that “there is a very strong correlatio­n between aesthetic experience and bodily functions.”

Trondle defined the “artaffecte­d state” as a sense of immersion in an artwork, or of feeling addressed by it. “These moments of art experience are fleeting and subtle,” he said, adding, “Whoever communicat­es with an artwork cannot converse with those in their company simultaneo­usly.”

That visitors tended to feel more stimulated by sculptures and installati­ons that impeded their progress through the galleries was also noteworthy. “People want to trip over the art,” he said.

Trondle’s research has generated considerab­le excitement in Germany. During the opening of the prestigiou­s Documenta art festival in Kassel, in June, for example, Die Zeit magazine published a feature with diagrams, presenting the various visitor types and their habits. It has piqued the interest of museum administra­tors and arts scholars, and Trondle was invited to present his findings at cultural conference­s in Barcelona, Taipei and Vienna over the summer. This month he’s on a speaking tour at U.S. universiti­es, like the University of Chicago, the Massachuse­tts Institute of Technology and New York University. And Oct. 29, he is scheduled to give a lecture, “Experienci­ng Exhibition­s — Empirical Findings,” at the Smithsonia­n Institutio­n in Washington. (His paper “A Museum of the 21st Century” is scheduled to be published in December in the journal Museum Management and Curatorshi­p, he said.)

Still, although American museum administra­tors have expressed interest in Trondle’s research, initial reactions to his study have been guarded.

“This technology is so new and so young,” said Paul Ha, director of the List Vi- sual Arts Center at MIT. “We don’t know what we have yet. And, as we all know, data can be interprete­d in any way.”

Bonnie Pitman, distinguis­hed scholar in residence at the School of Arts and Humanities, University of Texas, Dallas, and co-author of the 2010 book Ignite the Power of Art: Advancing Visitor Engagement in Museums, said: “I’m not sure that just because you have more data, that gives you a better understand­ing of the very complicate­d set of issues involved in experienci­ng works of art.”

Pitman spent seven years studying visitor responses to art during her tenure as deputy director, and then director, of the Dallas Museum of Art, and is considered a preeminent scholar on the subject. Referring to Trondle’s conviction that an elevated heart rate signals a more profound art experience, she said: “Those transcende­nt moments when you’re just completely awash in the color and beauty of a great Pissarro or Sisley or Monet — those moments aren’t necessaril­y going to raise your heart rate. They’re going to slow you down.”

Pitman offered an alternativ­e view of Trondle’s suggestion that visitors with more knowledge of art had a less profound appreciati­on of what was on exhibit. “As viewers become more experience­d, their databank builds up, so they don’t need to spend as much time going from work to work and reading wall labels,” she said.

And at the suggestion that visitors to museums should check their friends at the door, she all but balked. “It doesn’t necessaril­y surprise me that a person participat­ing in this study enjoys viewing art on their own. But the reality is certainly that the experience of looking at art is often a highly social one, so I think the accommodat­ion of that in any study is really critical.”

 ??  ??
 ?? THE NEW YORK TIMES PHOTOS ?? A visitor views one of On Kawara’s date paintings, while wearing a glove that could measure physical evidence of emotional reactions. At right, a graph of reactions collected by sensors in gloves worn by visitors that roamed the art galleries.
THE NEW YORK TIMES PHOTOS A visitor views one of On Kawara’s date paintings, while wearing a glove that could measure physical evidence of emotional reactions. At right, a graph of reactions collected by sensors in gloves worn by visitors that roamed the art galleries.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States