New York Post

‘SOCIAL JUSTICE’ IS KILLING SCIENCE

- JUKKA SAVOLAINEN Jukka Savolainen is a professor at Wayne State University. Adapted from City Journal.

NATURE Human Behavior, one of the most prestigiou­s journals for social-science research, recently published an editorial, “Science must respect the dignity and rights of all humans,” that generated tremendous pushback among academics and intellectu­als concerned about the spread of social-justice ideology into science.

Harvard psychologi­st Steven Pinker, for one, said the journal was “no longer a peerreview­ed scientific journal but an enforcer of a political creed.”

In short, the editorial took the position that scientific truth should defer to politics. The journal now considers it appropriat­e to suppress research that “undermines — or could reasonably be perceived to undermine — the rights and dignities” of people or groups, as well as “text or images that disparage a person or group on the basis of socially constructe­d human groupings.”

Researcher­s are urged “to contextual­ise their findings to minimize as much as possible potential misuse or risks of harm to the studied groups in the public sphere.” Anything that could be perceived as disparagin­g is now fair game for rejection or retraction.

The implicatio­ns for scientific inquiry and truth-seeking are clear. As the journalist Jesse Singal observed, an empiricall­y flawless study could be retracted under the guise of social justice: “What’s most alarming is that unless I’m missing something, research that is perfectly valid and well-executed could run afoul of these guidelines.”

But such behavior already occurs. Sometimes, studies that offend social-justice orarticle thodoxy are assigned a “flaw” of some kind — usually one that would be treated as minor had the results been different — and rejected on that pretextual basis.

The psychologi­st Lee Jussim has coined the term rigorus mortis selectivus to describe the widespread practice among social scientists to denounce research one dislikes using criteria that are ostensibly scientific but never applied to politicall­y congenial research.

Other times, studies that manage to penetrate the literature are seized upon by observers who scrutinize every aspect of the research using unreasonab­le criteria. Because no study is perfect, it is always possible to find some limitation to justify a cancellati­on campaign. Consider two recent examples:

One 2020 study suggested that junior female scientists benefit from collaborat­ing with male mentors. The publicatio­n of this article in Nature Communicat­ions (another journal in the prestigiou­s Nature franchise) brought a social-media firestorm and angry demands for retraction. Under growing pressure, the authors caved and “agreed” to retract the article on methodolog­ical grounds.

As the psychologi­st Chris Ferguson noted, the issues discussed in the retraction note were limitation­s “typically handled in a comment and response format, where critics of the article publish their critiques and the authors can respond.” The authors of the mentoring study had published an earlier study in the same journal showing evidence that “ethnic diversity resulted in an impact gain” for scientific articles. This un-retracted study had used a similar methodolog­ical approach to the retracted one, but nobody objected.

A 2019 study in Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, found no evidence of anti-black bias in police shootings. Initially, the PNAS editors were unwilling to entertain calls for retraction or even a correction. But after a critique in Science, they relented and published a reply-and-response debate.

The problem had to do with a poorly worded “significan­ce statement” — a publicfaci­ng research summary appearing outside the body of the article itself — claiming that “White officers are not more likely to shoot minority civilians than nonWhite officers.” Following additional scrutiny, PNAS published a correction in which the authors admitted to misleading language in one part of the significan­ce statement but stood by their research findings.

But in the feverish summer of 2020, and following extensive citations by City Journal’s Heather Mac Donald, the paper became dangerous. More than 800 academic luminaries, including Susan Fiske, a Princeton psychologi­st and wife of the relevant PNAS editor, signed a petition attacking the paper, causing the authors to agree to retract the paper that they had vigorously defended.

The PNAS editors admitted that their concerns were political: “The problem that exists now, however, is outside the realm of science. It has to do with the misinterpr­etation and partisan political use of a scientific after its publicatio­n.”

Why was that wording such a big deal? The authors themselves had already acknowledg­ed that the summary statement overhyped the results. If this is sufficient to retract a paper, then the wider body of social-science research is in danger.

Consider a recent sociologic­al study linking dog-walking to neighborho­od rates of violent crime. The study is entirely correlatio­nal and provides no causal evidence. Yet this didn’t stop the press release from declaring that dogwalking helped reduce street crimes.

In the words of one scientist, the Nature Human Behavior editorial codifies policies “that most social science journals already have.” In his 2014 book “The Sacred Project of American Sociology,” Notre Dame sociologis­t Christian Smith laments the discipline’s unwillingn­ess to come clean with the reality that pursuing specific kinds of social-justice goals is its central mission. As regrettabl­e as Nature Human Behavior’s new guidelines of may be, at least they express honestly how contempora­ry social science is actually practiced.

Indeed, scientific journals cannot afford to remain neutral — but they need to take a strong stand for the pursuit of truth, not for any political cause. Like democracy, scientific inquiry does not happen by default; it requires unwavering commitment among its participan­ts to play by the rules. It is not acceptable to retract or suppress a methodolog­ically sound study simply because you don’t like the results.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States