Pittsburgh Post-Gazette

There will never be certainty about Kavanaugh allegation­s

Memories are often impaired — by alcohol, trauma and even time

- Megan McArdle Megan McArdle is a columnist for The Washington Post.

My earliest memory is of the time I put a key into an electrical socket. Fine, cue the jokes, I electrocut­ed myself. And such a vivid memory that makes.

I can remember the keys, which were the color of an old penny, and how I slipped them off the bright yellow Formica of our kitchen island. I remember, too, the black Bakelite of the electrical socket. I remember noticing how neatly the one would fit in the other. And then I remember lying in our foyer while my mother, apparently on the phone with my pediatrici­an, shouted from the kitchen to my father: “The doctor says give her a glass of water!”

None of which happened the way I remember it.

I really did electrocut­e myself, but at 18 months, I was far too young to form durable memories. And how could I recall looking down at the top of our island when I was a toddler? It’s what psychologi­sts call “confabulat­ion,” the inadverten­t constructi­on of false memories. In my case, I probably heard the story more than once and, over time, created a memory to match, built from fragments of real memories — for example, how that island looked when I was 11.

Inadverten­t confabulat­ion is surprising­ly easy and common. At age 44, Elizabeth Loftus, one of the leading researcher­s on the fallibilit­y of memory, “remembered” that as a teenager she had found her mother’s body in the family pool. As with my false memory, the event was real; her mother had drowned when Ms. Loftus was 14. But Ms. Loftus didn’t discover the body. An uncle had mistakenly planted the idea.

Nonetheles­s, Ms. Loftus almost immediatel­y conjured up vivid details — her mother’s nightgown, a fireman giving Ms. Loftus oxygen to calm her down — that only went away after she spoke with relatives who said it was her aunt who found the body, and her uncle admitted he was wrong.

Ms. Loftus’ work, and her own experience, confront us with a difficult realizatio­n: False memories feel as real as true ones, even the really important ones. Neither joy nor trauma chisel indelible recollecti­ons into the bedrock of our consciousn­ess. They’re still just chalked on the pavement, prone to erasure or alteration by the rains of time. Which is why the Innocence Project keeps winning the freedom of prisoners who were misidentif­ied by eyewitness­es, often traumatize­d victims who believed absolutely in their misidentif­ication.

As you’ve probably guessed, this isn’t a random jaunt down memory lane. A year after the Senate fight over Supreme Court Justice Brett Kavanaugh’s confirmati­on, New York Times reporters Robin Pogrebin and Kate Kelly have published “The Education of Brett Kavanaugh.” The book aspires to provide better corroborat­ion of the allegation­s against Justice Kavanaugh than was available last year: Christine Blasey Ford’s claim that he tried to rape her when they were both teenagers and Deborah Ramirez’s accusation that he exposed himself to her when they were students at Yale.

Yet as the renewed fighting over Justice Kavanaugh this week suggests, we will never know which of these stories is true, or how much of them is true, because by now everyone’s memories are just too impaired — maybe by alcohol, maybe by trauma, certainly by time.

It’s not enough to find the accusers (or the defenders) sincere. Ms. Loftus honestly believed she found her mother’s body. No one has produced clear, firsthand, contempora­neous — ideally written down — corroborat­ion of the allegation­s against Justice Kavanaugh. Instead, we have secondhand reconstruc­tions of stories heard years ago.

Worse still, many of the people involved seem to have conferred with each other before the allegation­s went public, particular­ly in the Yale case; according to The New Yorker article that broke the story, Ms. Ramirez herself was initially unsure that Justice Kavanaugh was the man who had exposed himself to her and took six days to conclude that he was.

Trying to firm up fuzzy memories about decades-old events is an invitation to confabulat­ion. The media circus surroundin­g the Kavanaugh confirmati­on further muddled things; any witness who comes forward today must be presumed to have gathered significan­t details from the earlier coverage.

All of which means that we will never have any more certainty about the Kavanaugh story than we have right now — which is to say, none at all. We can either find a way to live with that fact, and each other, or partisans on both sides can keep tearing the country apart by insisting that they know the one clear truth about an irretrieva­bly murky past.

False memories feel as real as true ones.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States