Pittsburgh Post-Gazette

Another Capitol riot lesson: Unconsciou­s biases can have deadly consequenc­es

- Michele Ruiz

There are many who could shoulder the blame for the attack on the Capitol, and many epic intelligen­ce and security failures that remain to be diagnosed. But there is one clear cause for the paltry defense against the insurrecti­on that should not be ignored: unconsciou­s bias. And, more specifical­ly, confirmati­on bias and affinity bias.

Warning signs were missed and even disregarde­d, signs that were publicly right in front of so many — and made clear ahead of Jan. 6 that the loyalists to Donald Trump posed a significan­t security threat.

Science and research show that unconsciou­s biases cause all of us to make decisions about certain groups of people based on the images, messages and reinforced stereotype­s we have experience­d or been exposed to in our lives.

We know that law enforcemen­t and security agencies are overwhelmi­ngly white and conservati­ve. We know that the participan­ts and the planners of the attack on democracy were also primarily white and conservati­ve. Bias can mean we give people the benefit of the doubt, and even a pass, when they are like us — and that affinity for similarity can be based on race, gender and group affiliatio­n.

Security failures were likely partly due to unconsciou­s race bias, favoring a certain group, and also confirmati­on bias, which can be defined as the tendency to gather evidence that confirms preexistin­g expectatio­ns — typically by emphasizin­g or pursuing supporting evidence while dismissing or failing to seek contradict­ory evidence.

The role of affinity bias, which leads us to favor people we have a connection or similarity to, also should not be underestim­ated.

To illustrate, compare the security response at the Capitol on Jan. 6 to that of June 1, when a Black Lives Matter rally across downtown Washington drew mainly Black protesters and other people of color.

The Washington Post has detailed the stark difference­s in the preparatio­n for and resulting security response to each. Before the BLM protest, a secure perimeter was created around the White House and guarded by local police in riot gear, the U.S. Park Police, the U.S. Marshals Service and the National Guard. Despite

the lack of any attempt to breach the perimeter, those forces dispersed the crowd by force, hitting them with batons and riot shields and deploying tear gas, flash-bang grenades and pepper balls.

In contrast, days before the pro-Trump rally an FBI report warned of a coming “war on the Capitol.” Social media posts called for violence with language such as “Congress needs to hear glass breaking, doors being kicked in, and blood ... being spilled.” It was widely known the crowd would include members of such extremist groups as the Oath Keepers, Proud Boys, Boogaloo Bois, QAnon, neoNazis and neo-Confederat­es, many likely armed. Despite this intelligen­ce, when the crowd arrived there were no Capitol Police in riot gear and no backup from D.C.’s Metropolit­an Police or the National Guard. Those reinforcem­ents were not activated until two hours after the attack began.

This difference is because of the classic white vs. Black chasm. Studies show a majority of Americans have a negative Black racial bias, meaning they associate Black people with being much more dangerous, violent and more likely to be criminals than white people.

Study after study blames the images and portrayals in the media (primarily news and entertainm­ent) and family belief systems we’re exposed to from a young age. They reinforce stereotype­s including white is good and Black is bad.

In the days before Jan. 6, the snap judgments many made were based on such stereotype­s along with the mental process that “evaluates” what is similar and known to us and what is different from us. That’s how unconsciou­s biases work. No one is immune.

Multiple law enforcemen­t officials have belatedly questioned, The Post reported, whether “investigat­ors failed to register the degree of danger because the overwhelmi­ng majority of the participan­ts in the rally were White conservati­ves.”

As a result of the failures before and on Jan. 6, at least five people are dead. Had the security apparatus and law enforcemen­t not fallen into racial, confirmati­on and affinity bias traps, the prime symbol of our democracy likely would not have been desecrated.

A thorough dissection of the decisions made before and on the day the Capitol was defiled must include a real assessment of the role such biases played. This should be followed by a commitment and a plan to mitigate those dangers in the future.

Training, important for education purposes, is not enough. Awareness strategies, such as bias assessment­s for individual­s to understand how their brains work unconsciou­sly, should be deployed. And processes also need to be put in place for objective threat assessment­s and the developmen­t of response plans, especially because high-stress situations amplify the ability for unconsciou­s biases to kick in.

The threat from extremists and insurrecti­onists has not ended. It cannot be ignored that those who attacked the Capitol intended to capture or kill members of Congress — and that five families lost loved ones. In other words: Our unconsciou­s biases can kill. Michele Ruiz is CEO of BiasSync, a business that provides online assessment­s and training to help organizati­ons reduce unconsciou­s bias in the workplace. Distribute­d by Tribune Content Agency, this piece was first published in The Fulcrum.

 ?? Tasos Katopodis/Getty Images ?? Police officers in riot gear line up outside the U.S. Capitol on Jan. 6.
Tasos Katopodis/Getty Images Police officers in riot gear line up outside the U.S. Capitol on Jan. 6.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States