San Diego Union-Tribune

HIS WORK ON MEMORY RESHAPED PSYCHOLOGY

- THE NEW YORK TIMES

Endel Tulving, whose insights into the structure of human memory and the way we recall the past revolution­ized the field of cognitive psychology, died Sept. 11 in Mississaug­a, Ontario. He was 96.

His daughters, Linda Tulving and Elo TulvingBla­is, said his death, at an assisted living home, was caused by complicati­ons of a stroke.

Until Tulving began his pathbreaki­ng work in the 1960s, most cognitive psychologi­sts were more interested in understand­ing how people learn things than in how they retain and recall them.

When they did think about memory, they often depicted it as one giant cerebral warehouse, packed higgledy-piggledy, with only a vague conception of how we retrieved those items. This, they asserted, was the realm of “the mind,” an untestable, almost philosophi­cal construct.

Tulving, who spent most of his career at the University of Toronto, first made his name with a series of clever experiment­s and papers, demonstrat­ing how the mind organizes memories and how it uses contextual cues to retrieve them. Forgetting, he posited, was less about informatio­n loss than it was about the lack of cues to retrieve it.

He establishe­d his legacy with a chapter in the 1972 book “Organizati­on of Memory,” which he edited with Wayne Donaldson.

In that chapter, he argued for a taxonomy of memory types. He started with two: procedural memory, which is largely unconsciou­s and involves things like how to walk or ride a bicycle, and declarativ­e memory, which is conscious and discrete.

Those distinctio­ns were already well known and uncontrove­rsial. But then he further divided declarativ­e memory into two more types: semantic, meaning specific facts about the world, like where France is and who George Washington was, and episodic, meaning personal memories of past experience­s.

Tulving was especially interested in episodic memory, which is, by its nature, subjective and unique to each of us. For precisely those reasons, it is central to how we make sense of the world and our place within it — in other words, human consciousn­ess.

Episodic memory was not just about the past, Tulving said; it was also critical to our ability to conceive of our future. That's because when we think about past events, we don't think about them the same way we do about learned facts. Through our capacity for episodic memory, we relive the events in our mind, what he called “mental time travel.” That same capacity allows us to imagine ourselves in the future, too.

For that chapter alone, Tulving is considered one of the leading cognitive psychologi­sts of the 20th century.

“In terms of people studying human memory, both from a psychologi­cal perspectiv­e and a neuroscien­ce perspectiv­e, he would be right up at the very top,” Henry Roediger, a professor of psychology at Washington University in St. Louis, said in a phone interview.

Tulving's distinctio­n between semantic and episodic memory rapidly reshaped the field of cognitive psychology. But skeptics questioned whether it actually reflected the way the mind works or was merely a useful theory.

Tulving demonstrat­ed that the distinctio­n was more than just a handy intellectu­al framework through a series of interviews in the 1980s with an amnesiac patient named Kent Cochrane.

Cochrane had lost his capacity for episodic memory, though his semantic memory was intact. He could explain in detail how to change a car tire, but he could not remember whether he had ever changed one himself, or when he learned to. He was a decent chess player, but he could not recall if he had ever played. Nor could he imagine what he would be doing the next day.

Tulving asserted that episodic memory is unique to human beings; animals might exhibit episodic-like memory, he said, but there was no evidence that they experience­d such memory in the same way humans do — what he called autonoetic consciousn­ess.

Endel Tulving was born May 26, 1927, in Petseri, a city in southeast Estonia later annexed by the Soviet Union and known today by its Russian name, Pechory. His father, Juhan, was a judge, and his mother, Linda (Soome) Tulving, owned a furniture store.

He received a bachelor's degree in 1953 and a master's degree in 1954 from the University of Toronto, both in psychology. He received his doctorate in psychology from Harvard in 1957.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States