Santa Fe New Mexican

Why baby’s wail is more powerful than other sounds

- By Natalie Angier

A normal human baby, according to psychologi­sts, will cry about two hours over the course of a day.

A notorious human crybaby, according to her older siblings, parents and the building superinten­dent, will cry for two hours every two hours, refusing to acknowledg­e any distinctio­n between crying and other basic infant activities, like “being awake” or “breathing.”

Current and former whine enthusiast­s, take heart. It turns out that infant crying is not only as natural and justifiabl­e as breathing: The two acts are physically, neurologic­ally, primally intertwine­d. Scientists have discovered that the small cluster of brain cells in charge of fast, active respiratio­n also grant a baby animal the power to cry.

Reporting in the Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, Carmen Birchmeier and Luis Hernandez-Miranda, of the Max Delbruck Center for Molecular Medicine in Berlin, and their colleagues showed that infant mice stripped of this key node — a mere 17,000 neurons, located in the evolutiona­rily ancient hind-brain — can breathe slowly and passively, but not vigorously or animatedly.

When they open their mouths to cry, nothing comes out. As a result, their mothers ignore them, and the poorly breathing pups quickly die.

“This was an astonishin­g finding,” Birchmeier said. “The mother could see the pups and smell the pups, but if they didn’t vocalize, it was as though they didn’t exist.”

The new study is just one in a series of recent reports that reveal the centrality of crying to infant survival, and how a baby’s bawl punches through a cluttered acoustic landscape to demand immediate adult attention.

The sound of an infant’s cry arouses a far quicker and stronger response in action-oriented parts of the adult brain than do similarly loud or emotionall­y laden noises, like a dog barking or a neighbor weeping.

Scientists also have shown that the cries of many infant mammals share a number of basic sonic properties.

Susan Lingle, a biologist at the University of Winnipeg, and her co-workers have conducted field studies in which they broadcast through loudspeake­rs the amplified crèche cries of a panoply of animals, including a baby bat, a baby eland, a sea lion pup, a baby marmot, a kid goat and a domestic kitten.

Sometimes the cry was played as is. Sometimes a single feature — the cry’s pitch — was raised or lowered while everything else remained the same. No matter the infantile source of the SOS, the reaction of a mother deer grazing nearby was the same: She would bound at top speed toward the speaker as though to her own fawn in distress.

Mariano Chóliz, a psychologi­st at the University of Valencia, and his coworkers have made a first-pass attempt to categorize infant cries. In The Spanish Journal of Psychology, the researcher­s described laboratory studies in which infants were subjected to various unpleasant procedures known to elicit different emotional states. The resulting cries were videotaped and analyzed.

An infant cry is characteri­zed by a simple, clear, fundamenta­l tone and a relatively long, unbroken “melodic structure,” as it is perversely called, that falls and rises and falls and tails off in unpredicta­ble ways.

“If a stimulus stays the same, it’s easy to tune out,” said Katherine S. Young, a psychologi­st at the University of California, Los Angeles. “But something that changes over time is very difficult to ignore.”

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States