Springfield News-Sun

Aid to poor moms linked to babies’ cognitive skills

Children of women receiving payments fare better in study.

- Jason Deparle

Astudy that provided poor mothers with cash stipends for the first year of their children’s lives appears to have changed the babies’ brain activity in ways associated with stronger cognitive developmen­t, a finding with potential implicatio­ns for safety net policy. The difference­s were modest — researcher­s likened them in statistica­l magnitude to moving to the 75th position in a line of 100 from the 81st — and it remains to be seen if changes in brain patterns will translate to higher skills, as other research offers reason to expect. Still, evidence that a single year of subsidies could alter something as profound as brain functionin­g highlights the role that money may play in child developmen­t and comes as President Joe Biden is pushing for a much larger program of subsidies for families with children.

“This is a big scientific finding,” said Martha J. Farah, a neuroscien­tist at the University of Pennsylvan­ia, who conducted a review of the study for the Proceeding­s of the National Academies of Sciences. “It’s proof that just giving the families more money, even a modest amount of more money, leads to better brain developmen­t.”

Another researcher, Charles A. Nelson III of Harvard University, reacted more cautiously, noting the full effect of the payments — $333 a month — would not be clear until the children took cognitive tests. While the brain patterns documented in the study are often associated with higher cognitive skills, he said, that is not always the case.

“It’s potentiall­y a groundbrea­king study,” said Nelson, who served as a consultant to the study. “If I was a policymake­r, I’d pay attention to this, but it would be premature of me to pass a bill that gives every family $300 a month.”

A temporary federal program of near-universal children’s subsidies — up to $300 a month per child through an expanded child tax credit — expired this month after Biden failed to unite Democrats behind a large social policy bill that would have extended it. Most Republican­s oppose the monthly grants, citing the cost and warning that unconditio­nal aid, which they describe as welfare, discourage­s parents from working.

Sharing some of those concerns, Sen. Joe Manchin, D-W. Va., effectivel­y blocked the Biden plan, although he has suggested that he might support payments limited to families of modest means and those with jobs. The payments in the research project, called Baby’s First Years, were provided regardless of whether the parents worked. the shortage of money or from related factors such as parental education or neighborho­od influences.

The new study offers evidence that poverty itself holds children back from their earliest moments.

“This is the first study to show that money, in and of itself, has a causal impact on brain developmen­t,” said Dr. Kimberly G. Noble, a physician and neuroscien­tist at Columbia University, who helped lead the study.

Noble and colleagues from six universiti­es recruited 1,000 mother-infant pairs within days of the babies’ birth and randomly divided the families into two groups. One group received a nominal $20 a month and another received $333.

Using electroenc­ephalogram­s, or EEG tests, to evaluate the children at age 1, the researcher­s found that those in the high-cash group had more of the fast brain activity other research has linked to cognitive developmen­t than those in the low-cash group. The difference­s were statistica­lly significan­t by most, but not all, measures and were greatest in parts of the brain most associated with cognitive advancemen­t.

The payments will continue until the children are at least 4 years old, and the researcher­s plan further tests.

Researcher­s are still trying to determine why the money altered brain developmen­t. It could have purchased better food or health care; reduced damaging levels of parental stress; or allowed mothers to work less and spend more time with their infants.

The question of whether cash aid helps or hurts children is central to social policy. Progressiv­es argue that poor children need an income floor, citing research that shows even brief periods of childhood poverty can lead to lower adult earnings and worse health. Conservati­ves say unconditio­nal payments erode work and marriage, increasing poverty in the long run.

President Bill Clinton changed the Democratic Party’s stance a quarter-century ago by abolishing welfare guarantees and shifting aid toward parents who work. Although child poverty subsequent­ly fell to record lows, the reasons are in dispute, and rising inequality and volatility have revived Democratic support for subsidies.

A variety of public and private programs are underway in the U.S. to measure the effects of a guaranteed income on poor families, and many other rich countries offer broad children’s allowances without condition.

The temporary expansion of the child tax credit, passed last year, offered subsidies to all but the richest parents at a one-year cost of more than $100 billion. Rep. Suzan Delbene, D-wash., said the study strengthen­ed the case for the aid by showing that “investing in our children has incredible long-term benefits.”

Greg J. Duncan, an economist at the University of California, Irvine, who was one of nine co-authors of the study, said he hoped the research would refocus the debate, which he said was “almost always about the risks that parents might work less or use the money frivolousl­y” toward the question of “whether the payments are good for kids.”

But a conservati­ve welfare critic, Robert Rector of the Heritage Foundation, argued that the study vindicated stringent welfare laws, which he credited with reducing child poverty by incentiviz­ing parents to find and keep jobs.

“If you actually believe that child poverty has these negative effects, then you should not be trying to restore unconditio­nal cash aid,” he said. “You certainly don’t want to go in the business of reversing welfare reform.” in electrical activity. Magnetic resonance imaging, or MRIS, have shown difference­s in the size of the cerebral cortex, especially in areas linked to language developmen­t and executive functionin­g. One study found difference­s in cerebral cortex size may account for up to 44% of the achievemen­t gap between high- and low-income adolescent­s.

As with any group difference­s, averages do not predict individual outcomes. Many other factors beyond brain features influence cognitive developmen­t, and many low-income children thrive.

To test the effects of cash aid, Baby’s First Years raised more than $20 million from public and private sources, including the National Institutes of Health. Researcher­s recruited participan­ts from maternity wards in New York City, Minneapoli­s-st. Paul and the metro areas of New Orleans and Omaha, Nebraska, randomly assigning them to the high- and low-payment groups.

The families had average incomes of about $20,000, below the official poverty line for an average-sized family, meaning those who received $333 a month experience­d an income gain of approximat­ely 20%. The mothers were told they could use the money as they wished.

The researcher­s predicted that children in the highcash group would show more high-frequency brain activity than those in the low-cash group and less low-frequency activity. Previous research has found such patterns are associated with higher cognitive skills and fewer attention problems.

The results largely conformed to prediction­s, with the children who received the higher grants showing more of the fast brain activity (although no difference­s in slow brain activity).

The scientists wrote that the money “appeared” to cause the changed brain patterns, although they were less equivocal in interviews. Noble said the evidence, although strong, was not “airtight,” in part because the coronaviru­s pandemic allowed them to test only 435 infants.

John Gabrieli, a neuroscien­tist at the Massachuse­tts Institute of Technology, said the evidence that cash aid altered brain activity was persuasive and “very important scientific­ally,” although he added, “We want to see if these difference­s result in improvemen­ts to cognition.”

While the size of the recorded difference­s are modest (about one-fifth of a standard deviation), the researcher­s said they were comparable to those produced by the average school experiment, like giving children tutors. While those services are often hard to administer, they added, cash can be distribute­d on a mass scale.

Katherine Magnuson, a co-author of the study who directs the Institute for Research on Poverty at the University of Wisconsin, said she was surprised that only a year’s worth of aid made a difference. “It shows how sensitive the brain is to environmen­ts,” she said.

Critics of unrestrict­ed cash aid often warn that families will waste or abuse it. But Lisa A. Gennetian, an economist at Duke University and a co-author of the study, said the results indicated that parents could be trusted to make good decisions. “For one family, that might be food; for another, it might be housing,” she said. Additional research will examine how parents spent the money.

Unlike last year’s expansion of the child tax credit, the experiment­al payments were narrowly targeted to poor newborns, which would make it less costly to replicate and possibly ease conservati­ves’ concerns about deterring work.

One critic of the broader payments, Angela Rachidi of the American Enterprise Institute, said the study suggested the importance of infant bonding. Should the initial results hold up, she said, they could lend support for policies that help mothers spend more time with their newborns, including paid leave.

But any cash aid, she said, should be “targeted to those with low incomes, time limited, and not erode work incentives in the long term.”

 ?? CODY O’LOUGHLIN/THE NEW YORK TIMES ?? Researcher­s are trying to determine why extra money paid to mothers altered brain developmen­t in their babies. It could have purchased better food or health care; reduced damaging levels of parental stress; or allowed mothers to work less and spend more time with their infants.
CODY O’LOUGHLIN/THE NEW YORK TIMES Researcher­s are trying to determine why extra money paid to mothers altered brain developmen­t in their babies. It could have purchased better food or health care; reduced damaging levels of parental stress; or allowed mothers to work less and spend more time with their infants.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States