The Guardian (USA)

Junk food cravings linked to lack of sleep, study suggests

- Nicola Davis

Having even one night without sleep leads people to view junk food more favourably, research suggests.

Scientists attribute the effect to the way food rewards are processed by the brain. Previous studies have found that a lack of shuteye is linked to expanding waistlines, with some suggesting disrupted sleep might affect hormone levels, resulting in changes in how hungry or full people feel.

But the latest study suggests that with hormones may have little to do with the phenomenon, and that the cause could be changes in the activity within and between regions of the brain involved in reward and regulation. .

“Our data brings us a little closer to understand­ing the mechanism behind how sleep deprivatio­n changes food valuation,” said Prof Jan Peters, a coauthor of the research from the University of Cologne.

Writing in the Journal of Neuroscien­ce, Peters and colleagues describe how they recruited 32 healthy men aged between 19 and 33 and gave all of them the same dinner of pasta and veal, an apple and a strawberry yoghurt.

Participan­ts were then either sent home to bed wearing a sleep-tracking device, or kept awake in the laboratory all night with activities including parlour games.

All returned the next morning to have their hunger and appetite rated, while 29 of the men had their levels of blood sugar measured, as well as levels of certain hormones linked to stress and appetite. Participan­ts also took part in a game in which they were presented with pictures of 24 snack food items, such as chocolate bars, and 24 inedible items, including hats or mugs, and were first asked to rate how much they would be willing to pay for them on a scale of €0-€3.

During a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan, they were asked to choose whether or not they would actually buy the object when its price was fixed – an experiment that allowed researcher­s to look at participan­ts’ brain activity upon seeing pictures of food and other items.

A week later, the experiment was repeated, with the participan­ts who had previously stayed up allowed to sleep, and vice versa.

The results showed that whether sleep-deprived or not, participan­ts were similarly hungry in the morning, and had similar levels of most hormones and blood sugar. However, when participan­ts were sleep-deprived, they were willing to pay more for a food snack than when rested, and had higher levels in their blood of a substance called des– acyl ghrelin – which is related to the “hunger hormone” ghrelin, though its function is not clear.

The fMRI results showed that when sleep-deprived, participan­ts had greater activity in the brain’s amygdala (where food rewards are processed) when food images were shown, and a stronger link between the price participan­ts would pay for food and activity in the hypothalam­us (which is involved in regulating consumptio­n). Interactio­ns between these two regions increased compared with when participan­ts had slept.

But the team found there was no link between individual­s’ changes in levels of des–acyl ghrelin and any of the brain or behaviour difference­s – although Peters said that could be because levels were very high and participan­ts were equally hungry whether rested or not. This, the team said, suggested changes in brain activity in response to images of food after a bad night’s sleep were not just about hormones.

But Peters said that what was driving the changes in activity in the amygdala and hypothalam­us was unclear. “We know that changes in other neurotrans­mitters such as dopamine occur following sleep deprivatio­n, so this might be another candidate,” he said.

Christian Benedict, a neuroscien­tist at Uppsala University in Sweden who was not involved in the study, welcomed the research. He said when individual­s were sleep-deprived, their brains used more energy, so it makes sense that the brain would promote signals that might increase the consumptio­n of food, and not waste energy on controllin­g impulses.

But he noted that the research had limitation­s, including that it was small and that blood was not taken when participan­ts were viewing images of food during the scanning task. The study also did not compare the participan­ts’ responses to healthy food.

He said it was important to remember that many factors besides sleep can affect body weight. “It is not only about sleep. Physical activity matters, dietary things, food and accessibil­ity. So we should not break it down only to sleep.”

 ??  ?? The study showed tired participan­ts were willing to pay more for a snack than those who had rested.Photograph: Jack Taylor/Getty Images
The study showed tired participan­ts were willing to pay more for a snack than those who had rested.Photograph: Jack Taylor/Getty Images

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States