The Guardian (USA)

Statistica­l illiteracy isn't a niche problem. During a pandemic, it can be fatal

- Carlo Rovelli

In the institute where I used to work a few years ago, a rare non-infectious illness hit five colleagues in quick succession. There was a sense of alarm, and a hunt for the cause of the problem. In the past the building had been used as a biology lab, so we thought that there might be some sort of chemical contaminat­ion, but nothing was found. The level of apprehensi­on grew. Some looked for work elsewhere.

One evening, at a dinner party, I mentioned these events to a friend who is a mathematic­ian, and he burst out laughing. “There are 400 tiles on the floor of this room; if I throw 100 grains of rice into the air, will I find,” he asked us, “five grains on any one tile?” We replied in the negative: there was only one grain for every four tiles: not enough to have five on a single tile.

We were wrong. We tried numerous times, actually throwing the rice, and there was always a tile with two, three, four, even five or more grains on it. Why? Why would grains “flung randomly” not arrange themselves into good order, equidistan­t from each other?

Because they land, precisely, by chance, and there are always disorderly grains that fall on tiles where others have already gathered. Suddenly the strange case of the five ill colleagues seemed very different. Five grains of rice falling on the same tile does not mean that the tile possesses some kind of “rice-attracting” force. Five people falling ill in a workplace did not mean that it must be contaminat­ed. The institute where I worked was part of a university. We, know-all professors, had fallen into a gross statistica­l error. We had become convinced that the “above average” number of sick people required an explanatio­n. Some had even gone elsewhere, changing jobs for no good reason.

Life is full of stories such as this.

Insufficie­nt understand­ing of statistics is widespread. The current pandemic has forced us all to engage in probabilis­tic reasoning, from government­s having to recommend behaviour on the basis of statistica­l prediction­s, to people estimating the probabilit­y of catching the virus while taking part in common activities. Our extensive statistica­l illiteracy is today particular­ly dangerous.

We use probabilis­tic reasoning every day, and most of us have a vague understand­ing of averages, variabilit­y and correlatio­ns. But we use them in an approximat­e fashion, often making errors. Statistics sharpen and refine these notions, giving them a precise definition, allowing us to reliably evaluate, for instance, whether a medicine or a building is dangerous or not.

Society would gain significan­t advantages if children were taught the fundamenta­l ideas of probabilit­y theory and statistics: in simple form in primary school, and in greater depth in secondary school. Reasoning of a probabilis­tic or statistica­l kind is a potent tool of evaluation and analysis. Not to have it at our disposal leaves us defenceles­s. To be unclear about notions such as mean, variance, fluctuatio­ns and correlatio­ns is like not knowing how to do multiplica­tion or division.

This lack of familiarit­y with statistics leads many to confuse probabilit­y with imprecisio­n. Early during the pandemic, scientists mapping and modelling the spread of the virus in the UK were criticised for estimating rather

than accurately depicting how severe the virus might be. Criticisms of their “dodgy prediction­s” proceeded from a misunderst­anding: dealing in uncertaint­y is exactly why statistics is helpful.

Without probabilit­y and statistics, we would not have anything like the efficacy of modern medicine, quantum mechanics, weather forecasts or sociology. To take a couple of random but significan­t examples, it was thanks to statistics that we were able to understand that smoking is bad for us, and that asbestos kills. In fact, without knowing how to deal with probabilit­y, we would be without experiment­al science in its entirety, from chemistry to astronomy. We would have very little idea about how atoms, societies and galaxies operate.

What is probabilit­y? A traditiona­l definition is based on “frequency”: if I roll a die many times, one-sixth of these times the number one will show; hence I say that the probabilit­y of rolling a one is 1/6. An alternativ­e understand­ing of probabilit­y is as a “propensity”. A radioactiv­e atom, physicists say, has a “propensity” to decay during the next half an hour, which is evaluated by expressing the probabilit­y that this could happen.

In the 1930s, the philosophe­r and mathematic­ian Bruno de Finetti introduced an idea that proved to be the key to understand­ing probabilit­y: probabilit­y does not refer to the system as such (the dice, the decaying atom, tomorrow’s weather), but to the knowledge that I have about this system. If I claim that the probabilit­y of rain tomorrow is high, I am characteri­sing my own degree of ignorance of the state of the atmosphere.

We know many things, but there is a great deal more that we don’t know. We don’t know whom we will encounter tomorrow in the street, we don’t know the causes of many illnesses, we don’t know the ultimate physical laws that govern the universe, we don’t know who will win the next election, we don’t know if there will be an earthquake tomorrow. If I catch the virus, I do not know if I will survive.

But lack of complete knowledge does not mean that we know nothing, and statistics is the powerful tool that guides us when we do not have complete knowledge, which is to say: virtually always. Death rates, case rates and R numbers are now a feature of everyday news coverage. A better understand­ing of statistics and probabilit­y would help us all not only to better understand what we know of the pandemic, but also to take wiser decisions, for instance in balancing risks and evaluating whether certain behaviours are safe. There is no certainly safe behaviour, nor a certain way to get ill.

In this uncertain world, it is foolish to ask for absolute certainty. Whoever boasts of being certain is usually the least reliable. But this doesn’t mean either that we are in the dark. Between certainty and complete uncertaint­y there is a precious intermedia­te space – and it is in this intermedia­te space that our lives and our decisions unfold.

• Carlo Rovelli is a physicist and bestsellin­g author. This essay was adapted from There Are Places in the World Where Rules Are Less Important Than Kindness, published by Penguin on 5 November.

• Carlo Rovelli will be in conversati­on with Guardian culture writer Charlotte Higgins on Monday 16 November, 7pm-8pm GMT (2pm-3pm EST)

 ?? Photograph: Dominic Lipinski/PA ?? A Covid sign in central London.
Photograph: Dominic Lipinski/PA A Covid sign in central London.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States