The Guardian (USA)

Psychology is a powerful tool, but Britain’s Covid response has given it a bad name

- Stephen Reicher

For many years, psychology has largely been relegated to the “and finally …” section of the news, down there with dogs on surfboards and siblings reuniting after a lifetime apart. I recall, for instance, during the Scottish independen­ce referendum, being asked to comment on how political difference­s within families might lead to marital discord. Significan­t to those involved, no doubt, but hardly central to the story. Although issues that were central to the story – national identity, trust in government, decision-making under conditions of uncertaint­y – did involve a core psychologi­cal dimension, psychologi­sts and behavioura­l scientists more generally were never invited to comment on these.

The problem is that, although our society and popular culture are endlessly obsessed with the psychologi­cal, this is generally limited to how we act alone or in personal relationsh­ips. It rarely extends to how we act together, how we combine collective­ly and hence how we constitute a force that can alter the whole of society. So, when it comes to public policy, the discipline is irrelevant. Fine for the Big Brother House, less so for No. 10.

This has changed over the last year. At the beginning of the pandemic, it became quickly obvious that if we wished to control the virus, people would need to change their behaviours. As the UK government slowly lifts formal restrictio­ns on what people can do, it becomes ever more important that people are able to identify what risks remain, and to act on them.

But before we can dream of a glorious new dawn for psychology and the behavioura­l sciences, a couple of key objections need to be dealt with. The first is that, though behaviour may well be important, the sciences and scientists who purport to study it have got things so disastrous­ly wrong that they have proved more of a liability than an asset. The most obvious example of this is the sad history of “behavioura­l fatigue”: the notion repeated, at the start of the pandemic, that people would not be psychologi­cally equipped to deal with restrictio­ns on their behaviours and would only adhere for just so long. This argument was used to delay lockdown last spring. It was used to relax restrictio­ns at Christmas (as the Sun put it, otherwise there would be a “mutiny of mums”). It has been used in recent weeks to advocate loosening restrictio­ns as a third wave takes hold. It has probably cost many thousands of lives.

Not far behind in terms of destructiv­eness were more specific assumption­s about exactly what restrictio­ns a British public would or wouldn’t wear. For instance, it was argued that, unlike east Asian population­s, British people would never accept a rigorous testing regime with the need to self-isolate if infected. This led to more fatal delays in the introducti­on of measures that are of critical importance – most notably in building an effective test and trace system.

These were indeed catastroph­ic errors based on catastroph­ic misunderst­andings of behaviour. But, critically, they didn’t come from psychologi­sts and indeed were opposed by most psychologi­sts and other behavioura­l scientists. I well recall the horror in a meeting when we first heard about “behavioura­l fatigue” being used to oppose early action – we felt it was wrong, it was dangerous and it would end up being used to try to discredit our discipline­s. How right we were.

As for the orientalis­t fantasy that independen­t westerners won’t abide that which is accepted by passive Asians: this represents precisely what the study of prejudice has been critiquing since the second world war. The errors derived from the fact that decisions were made on the basis of “folk psychology”: assumption­s about human behaviour by non-psychologi­sts. The harm caused by these errors is not an argument against psychology. To the contrary. It is an argument for having trained psychologi­sts present when the key decisions are made.

The second objection is the polar opposite of the first. It isn’t that psychology and other behavioura­l sciences are ineffectiv­e and hence useless, it is that that these discipline­s are far too effective, which is precisely what makes them dangerous. Psychologi­sts become svengali-like figures able to

manipulate people to do anything and hence are enemies of democracy who must be kept at bay.

Certainly, there are some behavioura­l models concerned with manipulati­ng people without their awareness – “nudge theory” being a case in point. The core argument is that people don’t have access to the drivers of their behaviour so, rather than reasoning with them to do the right thing, the emphasis is on altering the “choice architectu­re” to make them do the easy thing. It is an approach that has considerab­le traction inside government – not least because it suggests that people can’t look after themselves and need an authority to look after them.

However, this behavioura­l economics approach was at odds with the approach of government advisers in the SPI-B behavioura­l advisory groupdurin­g the pandemic. Our emphasis was on the need to avoid a top-down approach, to root policy in a partnershi­p with the public based on respect and trust – in other words to create the conditions in which people will listen to reason. It is summed up in the mantra that effective policymaki­ng is a process of co-production with the public.

So, clearly, it isn’t right to simply call for more behavioura­l science and psychology in the formation of government policy. We also have to ask what sortof behavioura­l science and what sort of psychology. There is huge potential in an approach that understand­s how the power to address the pandemic (and future social problems) is not achieved by domination over people but rather by working with and through people.

In sum, if we learn from this time about the role that psychology (and the behavioura­l sciences more generally) could and should play in policy developmen­t, if we employ these sciences to facilitate the democratic involvemen­t of the public in policy initiative­s, we will be better prepared to handle the next crisis.

Stephen Reicher is a member of the Sage subcommitt­ee advising on behavioura­l science

 ?? Photograph: Matthew Horwood/Getty Images ?? ‘Although our society and popular culture are endlessly obsessed with the psychologi­cal, this is generally limited to how we act alone or in personal relationsh­ips.’
Photograph: Matthew Horwood/Getty Images ‘Although our society and popular culture are endlessly obsessed with the psychologi­cal, this is generally limited to how we act alone or in personal relationsh­ips.’

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States